Michael Tomz
Jessica L. P. Weeks
Keren Yarhi-Milo
January, 2018
Working Paper No. 1027
Public Opinion and Decisions about Military
Force in Democracies
1
Public Opinion and Decisions about Military Force in Democracies
Michael Tomz
Department of
Political Science
Stanford University
Jessica Weeks
Department of
Political Science
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Keren Yarhi-Milo
Department of
Politics
Princeton University
Version: January 7, 2018
Abstract: Previous research has used observational data to study the link between public opinion
and decisions about military force. We take a complementary approach, by using experiments to
examine two mechanisms—responsiveness and selection—through which opinion could shape
policy. We tested responsiveness by asking members of the Israeli parliament to consider a crisis
in which we randomized information about public opinion. Parliamentarians were more willing
to use military force when the public was in favor, and believed that contravening public opinion
would entail heavy political costs. We tested selection by asking citizens in Israel and the U.S. to
evaluate parties/candidates, which varied randomly on many dimensions. In both countries,
foreign policy proved as electorally significant as economic and religious policy, and far more
consequential than non-policy considerations such as gender, race, and experience. Overall, our
experiments imply that citizens affect policy by incentivizing incumbents and shaping who gets
elected.
2
I. Introduction
How does public opinion affect foreign policy in democracies? This has been a subject of
longstanding controversy, with profound implications for politics.
1
As many have documented,
democracies behave differently from autocracies in military disputes, trade, alliances, and other
forms of international conflict and cooperation.
2
Some theorize that these differences arise
because public opinion carries more weight in democracies, or because citizens in democracies
have unique preferences about foreign affairs. Accordingly, a growing body of research focuses
on the foreign policy attitudes of the mass public as a way to gain insight about international
relations.
3
If public opinion proved inconsequential in democracies, however, scholars would
need to rethink prominent explanations for the democratic peace, trade protectionism, and other
regularities in world affairs.
1
For reviews and recent contributions, see Aldrich et al. 2006; Baum and Groeling 2010; Baum
and Potter 2008, 2015; Gelpi 2017; Kertzer and Zeitzoff 2017; Milner and Tingley 2016; Reifler
et al. 2011.
2
E.g. Russett and Oneal 2001; Bueno de Mesquita et al. 2003; Mansfield et al. 2000; Leeds 1999,
2003.
3
E.g. Herrmann et al. 1999; Gelpi et al. 2009; Trager and Vavreck 2011; Levendusky and
Horowitz 2012; Johns and Davies 2012, 2014; Press et al. 2013; Chaudoin 2014; Kertzer et al.
2014; Stein 2015; Kertzer and Brutger 2016; Milner and Tingley 2016; Quek 2016; Spilker et al.
2016; Flores-Macias and Kreps 2017; Herrmann 2017; Prather 2017; Tingley 2017; Bell and
Quek 2018; Brutger and Kertzer Forthcoming; Tanaka et al. 2017.
3
The connection between public opinion and foreign policy is also normatively important.
If leaders routinely ignore public opinion on matters such as war, trade, and immigration, is this
apparent lack of representation a flaw that democracies need to address? If, on the other hand,
leaders follow public opinion even when citizens lack expertise, would it be better to insulate
elected leaders and the larger foreign policy establishment from public pressure? Before judging
whether democratic institutions ought to be reformed, we need to know how closely the foreign
policies of democracies reflect the will of the people, and why.
Although previous scholars have made valuable progress in studying the role of public
opinion, they have also faced roadblocks that reflect the limitations of available data. With very
few randomized experiments about the link between public opinion and foreign policy, scholars
have needed to rely on observational data. They have, for example, used historical records to
measure the correlation between public opinion and foreign policy decisions; analyzed surveys in
which citizens ranked foreign policy relative to other factors that might influence their vote; and
used post-election surveys to estimate how much foreign policy might have shaped electoral
outcomes.
These approaches, though insightful, face challenges inherent to observational research,
including selection bias, reverse causation, and confounders. Moreover, little research has
directly measured how leaders think about the connection between public opinion and foreign
policy. In this paper we take a fresh approach by presenting a series of experiments involving
policymakers and voters. Our experiments reveal how and why public opinion affects decisions
about military force, one of the most important dimensions of foreign policy.
We begin by distinguishing two pathways—responsiveness and selection—through
which the public could shape foreign policy outcomes (Stimson et al. 1995; Fearon 1999). First,
4
incumbent leaders may respond to public opinion out of concern that rebuffing the public could
be politically costly. Second, the public could select parties or candidates whose foreign policy
preferences reflect their own, whether or not those policymakers respond to public pressure after
taking office.
To test the responsiveness mechanism, we provide the first experimental evidence about
how leaders at the highest levels incorporate public opinion into decisions about foreign policy.
Our experiment presented 87 current and former members of the Israeli parliament with a foreign
policy crisis, in which we randomized information about how the public wanted to respond. The
experiment revealed that, all else equal, policymakers were more likely to support the use of
military force when the public was in favor. For additional insight we asked about the likely
consequences of making military decisions that conflicted with public preferences. The vast
majority of parliamentarians expected that the government would pay significant political costs if
it failed to heed public opinion.
To test the selection mechanism, we embedded experiments in surveys of citizens in two
different political contexts: Israel and the United States. Participants evaluated hypothetical
parties (in Israel) or presidential candidates (in the United States), which varied randomly in their
positions on foreign, economic, and religious policy, as well as on non-policy attributes. In both
countries, foreign policy exerted a powerful and consistent effect on voting preferences. Foreign
policy proved at least as important as economic and religious policy, and far more consequential
than non-policy attributes such as gender, race, and political experience.
Our studies build upon previous research in several ways. By using randomized
experiments to address problems of selection bias, reverse causation, and confounders, we put
causal inferences on firmer footing. By recruiting not only ordinary citizens but also
5
policymakers at the highest level, we are able to test key mechanisms and document how they
operate. And by combining data from Israel and the United States, we gain confidence that our
conclusions are likely to hold in diverse political settings. Overall, our study advances a
longstanding debate by providing experimental evidence that public opinion affects foreign
policy in democracies, both by influencing leaders once they take office, and shaping who gets
elected in the first place.
II. How Might Public Opinion Affect Foreign Policy?
In nearly all democracies, citizens delegate power to political representatives. Rather than
making policies themselves through public assemblies and national referendums, citizens elect
leaders to make policies on their behalf. Although representative democracy is often praised for
being efficient, it creates the potential for principal-agent problems: elected officials may
implement policies that contradict the will of the people. How can citizens in representative
democracies influence the foreign policy choices of their elected leaders?
Responsiveness
Most international relations scholarship has focused on ex-post solutions to the principal-
agent problem: after taking office, policymakers may feel pressure to respond to current public
opinion and/or expectations about future opinion. Policymakers might worry that ignoring
current opinion could prove costly during their time in office. Unfavorable public opinion could,
for example, make it harder to surmount institutional checks on war powers (Morgan and
Campbell 1991), raise funds for foreign interventions (Flores-Macias and Kreps 2013), and
amass the political capital to achieve other international and domestic goals (Howell and
6
Pevehouse 2007; Gelpi and Grieco 2015). Policymakers may also anticipate that pursuing
unpopular policies could contribute to defeat in the next election or reduce their margin of
victory, thereby weakening their mandate to govern in the future (Fiorina 1981).
Many influential theories in international relations are premised on the sanctioning
mechanism. According to selectorate theory (Bueno de Mesquita et al. 2003), public opinion
encourages leaders to be cautious about initiating military disputes. Knowing that angry voters
might unseat them, democratic leaders strive to avoid military operations that might be expensive
or unsuccessful. Similarly, Reiter and Stam (2002) argue that democratic leaders are careful
about initiating military conflict for fear that an adverse domestic reaction could stymie other
policy objectives and compromise future electoral outcomes.
4
The assumption that leaders respond to public opinion also underlies the literature on
domestic audience costs (Fearon 1994). Audience cost theory assumes that democratic leaders
are cautious about making threats, because they believe that domestic audiences—in
democracies, voters—will punish them for making threats and failing to follow through. This
insight has been extended to other commitments, including promises to aid allies (Leeds 2003)
and other forms of international cooperation (Leeds 1999). If leaders are not responsive to public
opinion, however, then this mechanism is unlikely to drive behavior in international politics.
Past scholarship has leveraged observational data to test the responsiveness mechanism.
These studies have produced important insights, but they have also faced challenges associated
with observational research, including selection bias, reverse causation, and confounders.
4
Baum and Potter (2015) argue that responsiveness is easiest in countries with robust opposition
parties and a free press.
7
Research has also been constrained by the paucity of data from the perspective of leaders who
make foreign policy. Below, we review four approaches to studying responsiveness and highlight
why it has been difficult to estimate the causal effect of public opinion.
The first approach involves estimating the correlation between public opinion and foreign
policy, for instance by observing whether leaders increase defense spending when the public
becomes more hawkish (e.g. Page and Shapiro 1983; Russett 1990; Bartels 1991). Scholars
employing this approach acknowledge, however, that “when opinion and policy correspond, it is
extremely difficult to sort out whether public opinion has influenced policy, or policy has
influenced opinion, or there has been some mixture of reciprocal processes; or, indeed, whether
an outside factor, by affecting both, has produced a spurious relationship.Problems like these
are unfortunately “a curse of nonexperimental sciences” (Page 1994, 26, 28).
A second approach compares foreign policy across institutional contexts. Scholars have,
for example, studied whether democracies, in which public opinion is thought to have more
sway, pursue different foreign policies than non-democracies (Bueno de Mesquita et al 2003;
Reiter and Stam 2002).
5
Although insightful, this approach offers only indirect evidence about
the role of public opinion and has been criticized on the grounds of confounding (Green et al.
2001). After all, democracies and dictatorships differ on various dimensions—many hard to
measure and control—that could produce differences in foreign policy even if the public played
no role.
5
Scholars have also compared types of democracies (Baum and Potter 2015) and contrasted
term-limited leaders with leaders facing re-election (e.g. Potter 2016; Carter and Nordstrom
forthcoming).
8
A third approach involves measuring the price leaders have paid for defying public
opinion. Research has examined whether democratic leaders historically suffered public backlash
or fell from power when they lost wars and enacted other unpopular foreign policies (Chiozza
and Goemans 2011; Croco 2015; Snyder and Borghard 2011). This research strategy is prone to
selection bias: when the anticipated costs are high, leaders will avoid bucking public opinion,
depriving researchers of the opportunity to observe the penalties for going against public
sentiment (Schultz 2001). Recognizing this problem, scholars have begun using experiments to
estimate how potentially unpopular decisions might affect public opinion (Trager and Vavreck
2011; Levendusky and Horowitz 2012; Kertzer and Brutger 2016). However, these experiments
have been conducted on citizens; we still know relatively little about whether leaders expect to
be punished/rewarded, and how they weigh these expectations in their decision calculus.
Finally, scholars could study responsiveness by finding out how leaders think about the
public. Few studies have adopted this approach, either by directly asking leaders how much they
take public opinion into account (e.g., Powlick 1991) or analyzing archival records to infer how
much public opinion affected foreign policy decisions (e.g., Foyle 1999).
6
While this approach
has many advantages, policymakers might not be fully conscious of their decisionmaking
criteria, and archival records might not allow researchers to isolate the role of public opinion.
Moreover, interviews and archival data could be affected by social desirability bias, i.e.,
policymakers casting their decisionmaking in socially approved terms. While it is difficult to say
whether policymakers believe they would be praised for responsiveness or criticized for heeding
6
Scholars have, however, interviewed elites to answer other questions about foreign policy (e.g.,
Mintz 2004; Herrmann and Keller 2004; Hafner-Burton et al. 2014; Bayram 2017).
9
the whims of foreign policy amateurs, the possibility of bias makes it important to complement
observational approaches with additional methods. Our experiments shed new light on the
responsiveness mechanism by providing direct data on the perceptions of elites and addressing
some of the limitations of observational approaches.
Selection
Less research in international relations has focused on a second solution to the principal-
agent problem: citizens can select leaders based on expectations about the foreign policies the
leader would pursue if elected. By empowering like-minded leaders (ones with foreign policy
preferences similar to their own), citizens can minimize the risk that representatives would want
to act against the public’s wishes. Voters can use various sources of information, including
campaign statements, party platforms, and past decisions, to infer whether candidates are “good
types,” i.e., “likely to be principled and share the public’s preferences” (Fearon 1999, 57).
Because selection takes place before leaders enter office, selection is an ex-ante solution to the
principal-agent problem.
Selection does not require voters to have detailed views on every specific policy issue.
Research shows that voters hold core foreign policy postures: broad, abstract beliefs—such as
about the proper role of military force or desired degree of engagement in world affairs—that
inform specific foreign policy preferences (Hurwitz and Peffley 1987a, b). Voters can use these
“superordinate beliefs” to guide their thinking on foreign policy issues and select like-minded
leaders (Holsti 2004, 55).
! It is important to recognize how selection differs from responsiveness. Selection reduces
the likelihood of shirking, not by incentivizing politicians with carrots and sticks, but by
10
empowering politicians whose own preferences match what the voters want. Thus, selection can
be effective even when citizens cannot punish politicians for stepping out of line, and even if
leaders are unresponsive to public pressure once elected. Nonetheless, the two mechanisms can
complement each other to tighten the correspondence between public opinion and foreign
policy.
7
The selection process has important theoretical and normative implications. A growing
body of scholarship shows that the ideologies, experience, and demographic characteristics of
leaders affect decisions about the use of military force (Horowitz, Stam, and Ellis 2015;
Saunders 2011; Colgan 2013). It is therefore vital to understand how citizens select the types of
leaders who will represent them. To what degree do voters use information about the foreign
policy preferences of candidates as a criterion when voting? How does foreign policy compare in
importance to other electoral considerations, including domestic policy, party affiliation,
experience, gender and race? If citizens assign significant weight to foreign policy when casting
their ballots, thereby selecting like-minded leaders, they should be able to influence international
relations whether or not leaders respond to additional public pressure after taking office.
Previous research about the selection mechanism has used public opinion surveys to
estimate how positions on foreign policy affected support for candidates in actual elections (e.g.
Page and Brody 1972; Aldrich, Sullivan and Borgida 1989; Gelpi, Reifler, and Feaver 2007;
Gadarian 2010). This approach, though useful, is susceptible to selection bias. Politicians have
incentives to choose platforms that will garner electoral support, so the platforms in real
elections rarely represent the full range of positions politicians could have taken if they were not
7
On the interaction between selection and responsiveness/sanctioning, see Fearon 1999.
11
acting strategically to maximize their votes. The strategic behavior of candidates can lead
researchers to underestimate the effects of foreign policy on voting. When citizens agree about
the direction that foreign policy should move, politicians face incentives to converge on that
dimension, effectively neutralizing foreign policy as a campaign issue and leaving citizens to
vote on other considerations. Thus, in observational datasets, foreign policy may appear
electorally unimportant at precisely the moments when it would have made the biggest
difference. More generally, the fact that political actors behave strategically makes it difficult to
know the counterfactual: how much support would leaders have garnered if they had not selected
their foreign policy positions with an eye toward winning the election?
8
A second challenge is reverse causation. In the 2004 U.S. election, proponents of the Iraq
War tended to vote for Bush, whereas opponents of the war generally voted for Kerry. One might
conclude that attitudes about foreign policy affected which candidate people supported, but
Berinsky (2009) argues that the relationship flowed in the opposite direction: support for Bush
versus Kerry affected whether individuals approved of the war. Unfortunately, with
observational data, it is difficult to estimate the weight of each causal pathway while holding the
opposite pathway constant.
Finally, confounders impede observational research about selection. When the foreign
policies of candidates are correlated with their domestic policies, as in many historical elections,
it is difficult to disentangle how much of the correspondence between voter preferences and
candidate positions is due to foreign policy, versus agreement on other issues. Similarly, the
8
Selection bias also affects research on other policy dimensions, complicating efforts to compare
across policy domains.
12
foreign policy positions of candidates could coincide with personal attributes such as gender,
race, religion, political and military experience, and place of origin. The confluence of many
electorally relevant factors makes it difficult to isolate the effects of foreign policy preferences.
Taking a different tack, researchers have analyzed polls about the “most important
problem” facing the country. In these polls, voters often rank domestic problems higher than
foreign ones, leading some to conclude that foreign policy has low salience and is typically
unimportant in elections (Almond 1950; Busby and Monten 2012). However, issues can be
electorally consequential, even when they are not ranked first in the public mind. Moreover,
surveys about the “most important problem” conflate two distinct considerations: whether the
issue is seen as important, and whether the status quo on that issue is seen as problematic
(Wlezien 2005). Foreign policy can be an important criterion for selecting leaders, not only to
deal with current problems, but also to avoid or address future problems.
In summary, due to the inherent limitations of observational!data,!we remain unsure about
whether and how public opinion affects foreign policy. In the remainder of this article, we!
complement observational studies by taking an experimental approach. Our experiments,
administered to policymakers and voters, test the responsiveness and the selection mechanisms
while addressing many problems that arise in observational research.
13
III. Testing Responsiveness#
We first examined whether politicians respond to public opinion when making decisions
about the use of military force. To answer this question, we recruited a unique sample of current
and former members of the Israeli legislature (the Knesset) and administered a survey with an
embedded experiment. The survey was fielded between July and October 2015. Of 288 current
and former MKs with contact information at the time of our study, 87 (approximately 30%)
answered our survey.
9
By focusing on the Israeli Knesset, we gained valuable insight into leaders who actually
make foreign policy. Israel is a parliamentary democracy in which elected members of the
Knesset also populate the executive branch. Many legislators are, therefore, directly involved in
decisions about the use of force. Moreover, Israeli election cycles are short, political turnover is
common, and coalitions are fluid, creating opportunities for many members to serve on the
cabinet at some point in their political careers. By surveying current and former members of the
Knesset, we are not only sampling lawmakers, but also accessing the beliefs of current, former,
and potentially future members of the executive branch. Consistent with these expectations,
participants in our study had impressive leadership experience. Roughly 67% had served on the
Knesset’s Foreign Affairs and Defense Committee; 34% had served as Prime Minister, Vice-
Prime Minister, or Minister; and an additional 9% had acted as served as a Deputy Minister.
We designed an experiment to test the effect of public opinion on support for a
hypothetical military strike. Each MK considered the following vignette:
“We would like your opinion about the following hypothetical scenario.
9
The appendix provides details for all studies in this article.
14
Ten armed terrorists emerged from an underground tunnel in northern Israel, close to the
border with Lebanon. The terrorists were planning to attack a Jewish town, take civilian
hostages, and bring them back to Lebanon. The IDF caught some of the terrorists, but
others escaped back into Lebanon. Several IDF soldiers were wounded during the
operation.
The cabinet discussed whether Israel should send special forces and planes to attack the
terrorist bases in Lebanon.
The security establishment is divided over whether Israel should carry out this military
operation. Supporters say the operation would punish the terrorists, reduce the threat from
the tunnels, and deter future attacks. Opponents say the operation would lead to IDF
casualties, would cause terrorists to retaliate against Israeli cities, and would escalate into
a large-scale military conflict.”
We then randomized information about public support for a military operation. Half of the MKs
were told that public opinion was strongly in favor of military action, while the other half were
told that the public firmly opposed the idea:
“The public strongly [supports/opposes] taking military action against the terrorists. The
media has covered the situation extensively, and polls show that more than 75% of voters
think Israel [should/should not] attack the terrorist bases. Citizens have started
demonstrating [for/against] the military action and sending letters to their
representatives.”
15
Having manipulated perceptions of public support for military action, we asked: “In this
situation, would you favor or oppose sending special forces and planes to attack the terrorist
bases?”
Figure 1 shows the percentage of MKs who supported a military strike, conditional on the
randomized treatment they received. Support for a military strike was nearly 16 percentage
points higher when a majority of citizens favored a strike, than when most citizens opposed a
strike. The estimated effect, though large, falls just shy of conventional standards for statistical
significance; the p-value associated with the treatment effect is .135 for a two-sided test and .068
for a one-sided test. When dealing with elite samples, it would be difficult to gain more
precision. To detect an effect of this size at a significance level of .05, we would have needed
responses from 290 MKs, more than the number of living MKs for whom we found contact
information. (Given these constraints, the sample is not large enough to support precise estimates
for smaller demographic or political subgroups.) We believe the treatment effect is informative,
even if not estimated with the precision one sometimes sees in large surveys of the mass public.
Figure 1: Effect of Public Opinion on Support for Military Strike
Note: Horizontal lines depict 95% confidence intervals.
16
When presenting the scenario, we randomized information about public opinion as a
whole, instead of varying sentiments within the respondent’s own political party. We did this to
avoid administering implausible treatments to MKs from predominantly dovish or hawkish
parties, who might have doubted the suggestion that most within their own party supported (or
opposed) military action. Having shown that MKs care about public opinion in general, though,
it seems likely that MKs would care even more about the views of their own co-partisans and
swing voters. In this sense, our experiment establishes a lower bound on how MKs would have
responded if we could have manipulated information about the opinions of specific voter
groups.
10
We supplemented the experiment by directly asking about the importance of public
opinion. “In general,” we inquired, “when you consider whether to use military force against a
foreign adversary, to what extent do you take domestic public opinion into account?” Responses
to this item, which appeared in a separate part of the survey, reinforce our experimental findings.
Only 3% of MKs said that they did not consider public opinion at all; 26% indicated that they
weighed public opinion to a small extent; 38% reported giving medium consideration to public
opinion; and the remaining 33% said that public opinion influenced their military decisions to a
large or very large extent.
10
We randomized information about current public opinion. Some MKs could have viewed the
treatment not only as a measure of current sentiment, but also as a harbinger of future opinion.
Nonetheless, the treatment effect might have been larger if we had explicitly randomized
information about what opinion would be on the eve of a Knesset election.
17
Were answers to this question tainted by social desirability bias? It is hard to know for
sure, but three factors help minimize such concerns. First, respondents were guaranteed
anonymity, reducing the incentive to respond in socially desirable ways. Second, it is unclear
whether any lingering biases would cause MKs to overstate or understate the role of public
opinion. Some policymakers might see value in appearing deferential to ordinary citizens, but
others might want to portray themselves as autonomous, relying on their own expertise and
values when making decisions about military force. Third, the responses corroborate our
experimental findings, by suggesting that public opinion exerts a substantial but not
overdetermining effect on elite decisions about war.
For insight about why MKs pay close attention to public preferences, we measured
perceptions of the consequences of failing to heed public opinion. “We would now like you to
think about Israel’s use of military force more generally. Please consider the following
hypothetical situations.” The first hypothetical situation involved an unpopular war: “Suppose an
Israeli government was considering whether to go to war against a foreign adversary. If the
public strongly opposed the war, but the government nonetheless decided to go war, please rate
the likelihood that each of the following events would happen in the short term.” We asked
whether the government would lose support in the polls; lose seats in the Knesset; find it difficult
to get support for other foreign and domestic policies; and fall from power. MKs indicated
whether each potential consequence was extremely likely, very likely, somewhat likely, or not
likely. In a similar way, we elicited expectations about what would happen if the government
failed to engage in a war that the public wanted.
18
Figure 2: Expected Consequences of Going against Public Opinion
Note: Percentage of MKs selecting each option.
8
42 42
8
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Lose support in polls
8
59
24
9
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Lose seats in Knesset
15
42
38
5
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Hard to pass other policies
41
49
8
2
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Government would fall
98
61
16
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
At least one is ....
Effects of Waging
an Unpopular War
14
42
30
14
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Lose support in polls
13
55
23
9
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Lose seats in Knesset
24
44
29
2
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Hard to pass other policies
57
37
6
Not
likely
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
Government would fall
94
53
17
Somewhat
likely
Very
likely
Extremely
likely
At least one is ....
Effects of Avoiding
a Popular War
19
As Figure 2 reveals, Israeli decision-makers anticipated adverse consequences both for
waging an unpopular war, and for avoiding a popular war. When it came to fighting an
unpopular war, nearly all of the MKs (98%) thought that at least one of the adverse outcomes
was somewhat, very, or extremely likely; 61% deemed at least one consequence to be very or
extremely likely; and 16% thought that at least one was extremely likely. If the government
stayed out of a popular war, 94% of MKs thought that at least one of the four adverse outcomes
was somewhat, very, or extremely likely; 53% thought at least one was very or extremely likely;
and 17% thought that at least one was extremely likely. Given the seriousness of the
consequences we listed and the high proportion of MKs anticipating these results, we conclude
that the repercussions of going against the public loom large for Israeli politicians.
One might wonder whether results based on Israeli Knesset members would generalize to
elites in other democracies. In some senses, Knesset members represent a hard case for testing
the responsiveness mechanism. The Arab-Israeli conflict has made decisions about military
action commonplace for Israeli policymakers, and mandatory military service has further
exposed Israelis to situations involving military force. Because terrorist attacks are familiar,
Israeli policymakers presumably hold a priori preferences about how to react, which could
reduce the effect of new information such as public opinion (Mintz et al. 1997). For these
reasons, our estimates would underestimate policymaker responsiveness.
At the same time, protracted conflict between Israel and its neighbors could make foreign
policy especially important to Israeli voters, increasing policymakers’ incentives to be
responsive. To shed light on this issue, we fielded experiments on the mass public in both Israel
and the U.S. Our experiments, discussed below, reveal that foreign policy is a weighty electoral
consideration for voters in both countries. This suggests that Israeli policymakers should not be
20
unique in expecting domestic political consequences for their foreign policy decisions.
In sum, we have presented unique micro-level evidence about the responsiveness
mechanism. In our experiment, members of the Israeli Knesset shifted their views about military
action in response to randomized information about public opinion. When asked directly, the
vast majority said that public opinion played a role in decisions about military force and
anticipated that rebuffing the public would bring serious political consequences.
IV. Testing Selection in Israel
We now consider the selection mechanism. To what extent do citizens select
representatives on the basis of foreign policy, and how does foreign policy compare to other
electoral considerations? As a step toward answering these questions, we hired iPanel, a
respected Israeli polling firm, to recruit a representative sample of 1,067 Jewish adults in March
2016.
11
The survey began by measuring respondents’ preferences on foreign, economic, and
religious policy. Since selection does not require voters to have detailed views on every specific
policy issue, we focused on overarching postures that guide citizens’ views. Our survey
measured hawkishness (a.k.a. militarism), one of the core dimensions structuring foreign policy
attitudes. “On matters of foreign affairs and security,” we inquired, “do you support a dovish
(left) or a hawkish (right) approach?” In our sample, 9% of participants classified themselves as
11
iPanel recruited subjects using benchmarks for gender, age, education, and area of residence.
For other studies using iPanel, see e.g. Ben-Nun Bloom et al. 2015 and Grossman et al. 2015.
We focused on the Israeli Jewish population because online polling companies cannot reliably
sample the minority Israeli Arab population.
21
“definitely dovish”; 27% said they were “more dovish than hawkish”; 39% deemed themselves
“more hawkish than dovish”; and 25% said they were “definitely hawkish.”
To quantify preferences about economic policy, we followed the Israeli National Election
Study by asking: “About the structure of economic life in the country, do you support a capitalist
or a socialist approach?” Responses were definitely socialist (11%), more socialist than capitalist
(53%), more capitalist than socialist (30%), or definitely capitalist (5%). Finally, to capture
views about religious policy, we inquired: “To what extent should the government require Jewish
religious traditions in public life?” The options were never (36%), sometimes (40%), often
(16%), or always (8%).
After measuring respondents’ policy preferences, we asked them to evaluate pairs of
political parties, which was appropriate given that Israelis vote for parties rather than individual
candidates. Our preface explained, “On the following screens we will describe a number of
political parties. The parties are hypothetical; they are not actual parties in Israel today. The vast
majority of candidates in each party are Jewish, and each party is expected to pass the electoral
threshold and enter the Knesset. Please read the descriptions carefully, and then tell us which
party you would prefer.”
We then displayed a table that described two parties, A and B, which varied randomly on
seven dimensions: foreign, economic, and religious policy; party size; and the military
experience, political experience, and gender of the party leader. We portrayed policies along the
same broad spectra we used to measure respondents’ preferences. Thus, each party’s foreign
policy was randomly assigned to be definitely dovish, more dovish than hawkish, more hawkish
than dovish, or definitely hawkish. Likewise, economic policies ranged from socialist to
capitalist, and religious policies ranged from never requiring to always requiring Jewish religious
22
traditions in public life. The economic and religious policy spaces were intentionally wide, to
avoid suppressing the apparent importance of those issues relative to foreign policy.
When describing the military experience of each party leader, we randomized whether
the leader had served only the mandatory minimum, had risen to the rank of junior officer, or had
attained the rank of senior officer. For political experience, we randomly drew an integer
between 0 and 30 to represent the number of years the party leader had been in national politics.
Finally, we indicated whether the party leader was male or female, and we mentioned whether
the party was (or was not) one of the three largest parties in the political system. We randomized
the order in which respondents saw these considerations.
We intentionally randomized each of the seven dimensions independently to produce
diverse combinations, including ones not common in current Israeli politics. This approach not
only avoided multicollinearity, but also allowed us to estimate the consequences of taking the
full range of policy positions, including ones that might be electorally disadvantageous.
We concluded by asking, “If you had to choose, which party would you vote for?” We
repeated the exercise with three additional pairs of parties: C versus D, E versus F, and G versus
H. Thus, each participant reviewed eight party profiles, giving us a large number of judgments
about an extremely rich political space.
12
12
Before asking respondents to choose between the parties, we measured attentiveness by asking
whether the parties were identical or different on each of the seven dimensions. The table
remained on the screen, so respondents did not have to answer from memory. Our analysis
focuses on the 1,067 (out of 1,277) respondents who correctly answered at least 85% of the
23
We simplify the discussion by presenting the effect of each attribute, averaging over all
the other dimensions of the experiment. To quantify the effect of gender, for example, we
measure how much better (or worse) parties with male leaders fared in our experiment,
averaging over all other characteristics the party might have, and over all characteristics the
opposing party might have.
We first consider how the foreign, economic, and religious positions of parties affected
support at the polls. The left side of Figure 3 shows the effects of foreign policy. As shorthand,
we represent each policy position with a number: 1=definitely dovish, 2=more dovish than
hawkish, 3=more hawkish than dovish, and 4=definitely hawkish. We estimated how these four
groups of voters responded to parties that agreed or disagreed with their own opinions.
The left side of Figure 3 shows that voters awarded substantially less support to parties
with distant foreign policy views, than to parties who concurred with them about foreign policy.
For instance, dovish voters (voters at 1) gave 37 points less support to hawkish parties (parties at
4) than to parties who shared their dovish ideal point. Likewise, hawkish voters (people at 4)
awarded 42 points less support to dovish parties (parties at 1) than to parties who sympathized
with their own hawkish preferences. These effects were not unique to extreme voters; moderate
voters penalized deviations, as well. Moderate doves (voters at 2) were 28 points less supportive
of parties at 4 than of parties at 2. Similarly, moderate hawks (voters at 3) were 30 points less
supportive of parties at 1 than of parties at 3.
attention checks. The appendix shows that the effects were similar, but smaller, when we
included inattentive respondents.
24
Figure 3: Effects of Policy Positions in Israel
The second and third columns of Figure 3 present analogous estimates for economic and
religious policy. To summarize the estimates and compare the electoral importance of the three
policy dimensions, the bottom row of each panel gives the average penalty that voters assigned
to a party that did not share his or her ideal point on that dimension (averaging across the twelve
ways that parties and voters could diverge). The average penalty for being out of step with the
voter on foreign policy was 20 percentage points, compared to 9 points for economic policy and
19 points for religious policy. We conclude that in Israeli elections, foreign policy is as important
as religious policy and more important than economic policy.
Foreign policy also outweighed the four non-policy attributes in our experiment. Figure 4
summarizes the average effect of the party leader’s political experience, military experience,
gender, and the size of the party. The top portion shows that, other factors equal, parties guided
by leaders who had been in national politics for more than five years performed substantially
better than otherwise comparable parties with less experienced leadership. The middle portion
0
1
-19
-37
-8
0
-17
-28
-30
-17
0
-5
-42
-30
-8
0
-20
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-60 -40 -20 0 20
Effect on Support (%)
Foreign Policy
0
3
-13
-28
-3
0
-15
-21
-17
-9
0
-5
-10
-6
12
0
-9
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-60 -40 -20 0 20
Effect on Support (%)
Economic Policy
0
-4
-20
-37
-7
0
-10
-21
-42
-20
0
-15
-35
-20
-1
0
-19
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Party at 1
Party at 2
Party at 3
Party at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-60 -40 -20 0 20
Effect on Support (%)
Role of Religion
25
shows that Israelis preferred leaders with extensive military experience. Overall, parties led by
former senior officers performed 3 points better, and parties led by former junior officers
performed 2 points better, than parties whose leaders had left the military after satisfying their
mandatory service. Finally, the bottom portion presents the average effect of gender and party
size. Israeli voters did not, on average, show a preference for male leaders over female ones.
Voters also threw 3% more support behind large parties than behind other otherwise comparable
parties that did not rank among the top three. This could be taken as evidence of strategic voting:
supporting large parties that might stand a better chance of forming governments and leading
coalitions.
Figure 4: Effects of Non-Policy Attributes in Israel
We ran many auxiliary tests, presented in the appendix, to confirm the robustness of our
findings. First, we checked that our results were robust to the specification of the dependent
variable. In addition to asking which party subjects preferred, we measured the strength of their
0
5
8
9
5
7
0
2
3
1
3
0-5 years
6-10 years
11-15 years
16-20 years
21-25 years
26-30 years
Mandatory only
Junior officer
Senior officer
Leader is male
Large party
Political Experience
Military Experience
Other Attributes
-5 0 5 10 15
Effect on Support (%)
26
preferences, and we asked them to rate each party individually on a scale of 0 to 10. When we
operationalized our dependent variable to take into account the strength of voters’ preferences, or
studied party ratings rather than the comparisons between the parties, our conclusions about the
absolute and relative effects of foreign policy remained unchanged.
Second, we investigated whether the effects of foreign policy were conditional on the
stances parties took on other issues. The average penalty for deviating from voters’ foreign
policy preferences was similar, regardless of whether the party took leftist positions on both
economic and religious policy, espoused right-wing positions on the economy and religion, or
took mixed stances. We also wondered whether foreign policy might be more consequential for
parties with moderate positions on other issues. To evaluate this possibility, we distinguished
parties with moderate positions (2 or 3) on both economic and religious policy, parties with an
extreme position (1 or 4) on either economic or religious policy, and parties with extreme
positions in both policy areas. Foreign policy proved highly consequential, regardless of the
party’s moderation or extremism in other areas.
We then checked whether the effects of foreign policy depended on non-policy
characteristics of the party or party leader. We found that foreign policy was equally important
for small and large parties, and did not vary according to the party leader’s military experience,
number of years in politics, or gender.
Finally, we investigated whether the weight of foreign policy varied across different
types of voters. In our analysis, foreign policy swayed subjects regardless of the voter’s gender,
age, education, income, military service, religiosity, and ideology. The appendix shows that the
effects were the same, if not stronger, when we restricted attention to the most politically
interested and involved members of society.
27
In sum, our evidence indicates that foreign policy profoundly affected support for parties
in Israel. Our experiments, which randomly and independently manipulated the foreign policy
positions of parties, allowed us to estimate the effect of foreign policy while addressing problems
of selection bias, reverse causation, and confounders. Foreign policy swayed voters regardless of
the parties’ other attributes, and mattered to voters across the social, economic, and political
spectrum.
V. Testing Selection in the U.S.
To assess the generalizability of our findings, we replicated our selection experiment in
the United States, with minor adjustments for the American political context. While both Israel
and the U.S. are established democracies, they differ in important ways, including electoral
system, size, geographic location, and threat environment. Finding similar results in the U.S.
would increase confidence that the effects of foreign policy generalize to other democracies.
In April 2017, Survey Sampling International recruited a sample of 1,420 U.S. adults,
chosen for demographic representativeness according to age, education, gender, income, race,
and region. As in the Israeli experiment, we began by asking subjects about their preferences
over foreign policy. “Some people think military force should be used frequently in U.S. foreign
policy. They are called ‘hawks.’ Other people think U.S. foreign policy should be based on
diplomacy, and the U.S. should rarely if ever use military force. They are called ‘doves.’ Which
approach to U.S. foreign policy do you prefer?” Roughly 15% said they were definitely dovish,
50% described themselves as more dovish than hawkish, 27% were more hawkish than dovish,
and the remaining 9% identified as definitely hawkish.
28
To capture preferences about economic policy, we inquired: “Some people favor
capitalist economic policies. They think the government should play only a small role in the
economy, and should let the market determine economic outcomes. Other people favor socialist
economic policies. They think the government should play a large role in the economy by
regulating businesses and redistributing income. Which approach to U.S. economic policy do
you prefer?” Around 5% were definitely socialist (large government role), 28% were more
socialist than capitalist, 39% were more capitalist than socialist, and 28% were definitely
capitalist (small government role).
Finally, to measure views about the role of religion in government, we asked: “How big
of a role do you think religion should play in shaping government policy in the United States?”
The options were no role (favored by 46% of respondents), small role (21%), medium role
(20%), or large role (14%).
We then presented a table with two hypothetical presidential candidates, A and B. The
candidates varied randomly in their foreign, economic, and religious policy positions, as well as
four non-policy attributes: home region (Northeast, South, Midwest, or West), race (White,
Black, or Hispanic), gender, and political experience (years in politics, from 0 to 30).
13
We
concluded by asking, “If you had to choose, which candidate would you vote for?” The options
were definitely Candidate A; probably Candidate A; probably Candidate B; and definitely
13
To control perceptions of religion, we portrayed all candidates as Christian. We intentionally
chose not to include information about the candidate’s political party in this experiment; below
we present a follow-up experiment in which we varied party affiliations.
29
Candidate B. We repeated this exercise three times, yielding data on four candidate pairings per
respondent.
14
Figure 5: Effects of Policy Positions in the U.S.
Figure 5 shows that foreign policy had powerful effects in the U.S. Dovish voters (people
at 1) were 32 percentage points less supportive of candidates with hawkish platforms (candidate
at 4). Other groups of voters reacted similarly, extending substantially less support to candidates
whose foreign policy views diverged from their own. The effects of foreign policy were not only
large in an absolute sense, but also comparable to the effects of economic and religious policy.
The bottom row of Figure 5 summarizes the average penalty for deviating from the voter’s ideal
point. On average, mismatches on foreign policy sapped support by 13 percentage points. The
14
As in Israel, we administered simple attention checks. We restricted our analysis to the 1,420
(of 2,051) respondents who answered at least 85% of the attention checks correctly. See the
appendix for similar patterns when we included inattentive respondents.
0
-8
-23
-32
0
0
-12
-21
-15
-7
0
1
-18
-18
-4
0
-13
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-40 -30 -20 -10 0 10
Effect on Support (%)
Foreign Policy
0
-2
-19
-22
1
0
-10
-17
-22
-15
0
-2
-34
-22
-3
0
-14
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-40 -30 -20 -10 0 10
Effect on Support (%)
Economic Policy
0
-10
-20
-36
-1
0
0
-18
-23
-2
0
-3
-32
-16
-9
0
-14
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-40 -30 -20 -10 0 10
Effect on Support (%)
Role of Religion
30
effects for economic and religious policy were similar: around 14 points, on average. These
findings confirm that the powerful effects of foreign policy are not specific to Israel.
Non-policy attributes mattered to a lesser degree. Figure 6 shows that region, gender, and
race had minor effects on preferences over presidential candidates, though voters rewarded
candidates who had spent more extensive time in politics by up to 9 percentage points, on
average.
Figure 6: Effects of Non-Policy Attributes in the U.S.
Finally, we probed the robustness of our findings (see appendix). As in Israel, the effects
of foreign policy remained strong regardless of the specification of the dependent variable;
candidate stances on economic and religious policies; the candidates’ home region, political
experience, gender, and race; and voters’ gender, age, race, education, income, religion,
religiosity, party affiliation, and home region. As in Israel, the effects were slightly stronger
among the politically active and those interested in politics.
0
4
6
8
9
9
0
2
2
1
0
0
2
10
0
1
1
4
5
0-5 years
6-10 years
11-15 years
16-20 years
21-25 years
26-30 years
F voter, M cand
F voter, F cand
M voter, F cand
M voter, M cand
Different races
Both hispanic
Both white
Both black
Different regions
Both Northeast
Both South
Both Midwest
Both West
Experience
Gender
Race
Region
-5 0 5 10 15
Effect on Support (%)
31
For the experiment described above, we intentionally omitted information about
candidates’ partisan affiliations because our goal was to isolate the effect of policy positions. If
we had independently varied party labels, which convey information about policy, many
candidates’ policy positions would have contradicted their party membership. For example,
respondents would have encountered Republican candidates advocating socialist economic
policies, and Democratic candidates supporting a major role for religion in politics—
combinations that are not very plausible today. Subjects who encountered such candidates might
doubt their policy positions, party allegiance, or both.
Nonetheless, it is interesting to ask whether foreign policy influences support for
candidates when voters know the party affiliations of candidates. To answer this question, we
designed a follow-up experiment, fielded via MTurk in September of 2017 on a diverse sample
of 1,462 U.S. adults.
15
This experiment retained the core design of the main study, with a few
necessary changes. First, we presented pairs of candidates with competing party affiliations:
Candidate A was a Democrat and Candidate B was a Republican, or vice versa.
16
Second, to
avoid potential contradictions between party and policy, we held all economic policies constant
at “more capitalist than socialist” and set all religious policies as allowing a “small role” for
religion in shaping government policy. As in our previous experiment, we independently
randomized gender, race, political experience, and foreign policy.
15
In our sample, 31% identified as Democrats, 37% identified as Republicans, and the remainder
were independent or affiliated with other parties.
16
We dropped the mention of the home region so that the number of candidate attributes (policy
positions and non-policy attributes) remained at seven, as in our main experiment.
32
As expected, partisanship mattered greatly. Other factors equal, Democratic voters
favored candidates from their own party 76% of the time and Republicans favored co-partisans
79% of the time. Independents were indifferent to party, choosing the Democratic candidate just
under half of the time. Personal attributes such as political experience, gender, and race showed
similar patterns to our main U.S. study (see the appendix).
Despite the powerful pull of party, foreign policy positions remained important. Figure 7
shows the effect of foreign policy when subjects chose between Democratic and Republican
candidates. As in our main study, voters were much more likely to support candidates whose
foreign policy positions matched their own. For example, dovish voters (voters at 1) were 38
percentage points less likely to favor strong hawks (candidates at 4). On average, voters were 16
points less likely to prefer candidates whose foreign policy positions diverged from their own.
The appendix further breaks down the effects by voters’ party affiliation, showing that
candidates’ foreign policy positions matter regardless of subjects’ partisanship. The effects of
foreign policy were not unique to Independent voters: Democrats and Republicans were often
willing to cross party lines on the basis of foreign policy.
33
Figure 7: Effect of Foreign Policy in the U.S., with Party Labels
Our experiments revealed that foreign policy had a large effect on support for presidential
candidates in the U.S., even when voters chose between a Democratic and a Republican. One
might wonder, however, whether we fielded our surveys at a time when foreign policy was
unusually salient, e.g. important, to American voters. To assess this possibility, we turned to the
most common measure of the relative importance of policy issues in the U.S.: survey data about
the “most important problem” facing the country. While these data confound the importance of
issues with their status as a “problem” (Wlezien 2005), readers might be interested in seeing how
2017 compares to other years on this widely-used indicator of foreign policy salience. We
therefore tallied the percentage of respondents naming a foreign-policy-related issue as the most
important problem facing the U.S. in April and September 2017, the months when we fielded our
0
-9
-27
-38
-7
0
-16
-30
-21
-13
0
-6
-19
-6
-2
0
-16
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Cand at 1
Cand at 2
Cand at 3
Cand at 4
Average
Penalty
Voter at 1
Voter at 2
Voter at 3
Voter at 4
-40 -30 -20 -10 0 10
Effect on Support (%)
34
U.S. surveys, and compared them to historical data.
17
Around 15% (April) and 12% (September)
of respondents named a foreign policy issue as the most important problem facing the country.
This compares to a median of 23% between the years 1939 and 2015.
18
Thus, in 2017 the
perceived importance of foreign policy (as measured by most important problem) was lower than
usual, increasing our confidence that the powerful effects of foreign policy uncovered in our
experiments are not unique to 2017, and may often be even larger.
VI. Conclusion
There has been much debate about whether and how public opinion influences foreign
policy. Previous research has made valuable contributions but faced challenges inherent to
observational research. In this article we complement previous work by using randomized
experiments to test two mechanisms—responsiveness and selection—through which public
opinion could affect foreign policy. Our experiments, administered to both elites and voters, shed
new light on an important debate while addressing problems of selection bias, reverse causation,
and confounding that have hampered observational research.
Our experiments confirm that politics does not stop at the water’s edge. We tested
responsiveness by administering an experiment to members of the Israeli Knesset, and found that
17
We used the Gallup aggregate survey data (news.gallup.com/poll/1675/most-important-
problem.aspx). To identify issues related to foreign policy (external relations), we applied the
coding rules described in Heffington et al. (2017). Where Gallup reported that less than 0.5% of
respondents had given an answer choice, we rounded down to 0.
18
We used the Marpor1_perc variable in the annual time series by Heffington et al. (2017).
35
views about military action changed as a function of randomized information about public
support for a military strike. Moreover, when asked directly, most Israeli policymakers reported
that bucking public opinion would entail significant political costs.
To test for selection, we fielded experiments on the mass public in both Israel and the
United States, and found that voters treated foreign policy as a major criterion for voting. When
foreign policy positions were exogenously assigned, voters strongly preferred parties or
candidates whose foreign policy positions matched their own. In both countries, foreign policy
was as important as economic or religious policies, and far more potent than non-policy
considerations such as gender, race, or political experience. Moreover, in the United States,
many voters crossed party lines to support candidates who shared their foreign policy views.
These findings have important empirical, theoretical, and normative implications.
Broadly, our findings suggest that scholars can gain insight into foreign policy by studying the
opinions of ordinary citizens, an approach taken by a large recent wave of international relations
scholarship. By demonstrating that leaders respond to public pressure when making foreign
policy, and that citizens strive to select leaders whose foreign policy preferences match their
own, this article underscores the value of recent efforts to measure and explain the preferences of
citizens.
Moreover, our experiments bolster many influential theories of international relations in
which citizens play a prominent role in shaping foreign policy. The long list includes, for
example, work on liberalism (Moravcsik 1997), the democratic peace (Doyle 1986; Maoz and
Russett 1993), selectorate theory (Bueno de Mesquita et al. 2003), audience costs (Fearon 1994),
diversionary war (Gelpi 1997), the democratic advantage (Lake 1992; Reiter and Stam 2002),
two-level games (Putnam 1988; Milner 1997), and theories about how citizens help enforce
36
international law (Dai 2005; Simmons 2009) and promote international cooperation (Leeds 1999;
McGillivray and Smith 2008). These theories, and many more, presume a strong connection
between domestic opinion and foreign policy.
Our evidence that foreign policy plays an important role in elections also has implications
for theories about the effect of leaders on foreign policy. Recent work demonstrates that the
ideologies, experience, and demographic characteristics of leaders shape decisions about war
(Horowitz, Stam, and Ellis 2015; Saunders 2011; Colgan 2013). We contribute to this growing
literature by uncovering the criteria citizens use to select their leaders. Our experiments reveal
that, when voting, citizens place heavy weight on the foreign policy positions of parties and
presidential contenders. Citizens also select on the basis of experience and demographic
variables such as gender and race, albeit to a lesser degree. By illuminating the electoral
processes that contribute to variation in leader type, we show an important way in which the
public can influence international relations.
Our findings suggest many productive avenues for future research. We focused on public
opinion about security policy, but scholars could adapt our experimental approach to explore the
effect of public opinion on decisions about international trade, foreign aid, and climate change.
19
Following the lead of Trager and Vavreck (2011), scholars could also use experiments to study
how voters select leaders on the basis of foreign policy outcomes, while holding constant the
leader’s choice of strategy.
Future scholarship could further investigate to what extent political elites can manage
19
For reviews of research on these policy areas, see Guisinger 2017; Milner and Tingley 2013;
Bernauer 2013.
37
public preferences and perceptions. While some scholars have argued that elites can shape what
the public thinks (e.g. Berinsky 2009; Saunders 2015), others do not see public opinion as being
at the command of policymakers (Gelpi 2010; Kertzer and Zeitzoff 2017).
20
New experiments,
building on the methods described here, could shed additional light on these questions.
Our findings also raise questions about whether responsiveness and selection are more
powerful ion some political contexts than in others. For instance, leaders may be more
responsive when they are eligible for re-election and face serious political competition, leading
to differences between parliamentary and presidential systems and dual versus multiparty
environments (Risse-Kappen 1991; Baum and Potter 2015). Responsiveness and selection could
also be weaker when politics are highly polarized (Jacobs and Shapiro 2000; Guisinger and
Saunders 2017). Finally, both responsiveness and selection should depend on public attention to
foreign policy—e.g. the salience of foreign policy—which could vary both across and within
countries as a function of geographic location, security relationships, and political structures.
These are rich areas for future research.
Our experiments also raise important normative questions. We found that citizens use
foreign policy as a criterion in choosing their representatives, and that representatives are
sensitive to public attitudes once in power. This is good news from the standpoint of political
representation. However, the powerful role of public opinion underscores the importance of
educating citizens to make sound decisions about weighty matters such as the use of military
force abroad. Future research should deepen our knowledge of how voters form opinions about
20
Still others have found that elite cues are important only in some issue areas (Guisinger and
Saunders 2017) or in some time periods (Baum and Groeling 2010).
38
foreign affairs, who they look to for information, and how to increase the likelihood that the
public will make sensible judgments about international politics.
39
References
Aldrich, John H., John L. Sullivan, and Eugene Borgida. 1989. “Foreign Affairs and Issue
Voting.” American Political Science Review 83(1):123–41.
Aldrich, John H., Christopher Gelpi, Peter Feaver, Jason Reifler, and Kristin Thompson Sharp.
2006. “Foreign Policy and the Electoral Connection.” Annual Review of Political Science
9(1):477–502.
Almond, Gabriel A. 1950. The American People and Foreign Policy. New York: Harcourt
Brace.
Arian, Asher, André Krouwel, Mark Pol, and Raphael Ventura. 2011. “The Election Compass:
party Profiling and Voter Attitudes.” In The Elections in Israel 2009, eds. Asher Arian and
Michal Shamir, 275–98. New Brunswick: Transaction.
Bartels, Larry M. 1991. “Constituency Opinion and Congressional Policy Making.” American
Political Science Review 85(2):457–74.
Ben-Nun Bloom, Pazit, Gizem Arikan, and Marie Courtemanche. 2015. “Religious Social
Identity, Religious Belief, and Anti-Immigration Sentiment.” American Political Science
Review 109(2):203–21.
Baum, Matthew A., and Tim J. Groeling. 2010. War Stories. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Baum, Matthew A., and Philip B.K. Potter. 2008. “Relationships between Mass Media, Public
Opinion, and Foreign Policy.” Annual Review of Political Science 11:39–65.
Baum, Matthew A., and Philip B.K. Potter. 2015. War and Democratic Constraint. Princeton:
Princeton University Press.
Bayram, A. Burcu. 2017. “Due Deference.” International Organization 71(S1):S137–63.
40
Bell, Mark S., and Kai Quek. 2018. “Authoritarian Public Opinion and the Democratic Peace.”
International Organization 72(1).
Berinsky, Adam J. 2009. In Time of War. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bernauer, Thomas. 2013. “Climate Change Politics.” Annual Review of Political Science
16:421–48.
Brutger, Ryan, and Joshua D. Kertzer. Forthcoming. “A Dispositional Theory of Reputation
Costs.” International Organization.
Bueno de Mesquita, Bruce, Alastair Smith, Randolph M. Siverson, and James D. Morrow. 2003.
The Logic of Political Survival. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Busby, Joshua W., and Jonathan Monten. 2012. “Republican Elites and Foreign Policy
Attitudes.” Political Science Quarterly 127(1):105–42.
Carter, Jeff, and Timothy Nordstrom. Forthcoming. “Term Limits, Leader Preferences, and
Interstate Conflict.” International Studies Quarterly.
Chaudoin, Stephen. 2014. “Promises or Policies? An Experimental Analysis of International
Agreements and Audience Reactions.” International Organization 68(1):235–56.
Chiozza, Giacomo, and Hein Erich Goemans. 2011. Leaders and International Conflict.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Colgan, Jeff D. 2013. Petro-Aggression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Croco, Sarah E. 2015. Peace at What Price? New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dai, Xinyuan. 2005. “Why Comply? The Domestic Constituency Mechanism.” International
Organization 59(2):363–98.
Doyle, Michael W. 1986. “Liberalism and World Politics.” American Political Science Review
80(4):1151–69.
41
Fearon, James D. 1994. “Domestic Political Audiences and the Escalation of International
Disputes.” American Political Science Review 88(3):577–92.
Fearon, James D. 1999. “Electoral Accountability and the Control of Politicians.” In Democracy,
Accountability, and Representation, eds. Przeworski, Stokes, and Manin. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Fiorina, Morris. 1981. Retrospective Voting in American National Elections. New Haven: Yale
University Press.
Flores-Macías, Gustavo A., and Sarah E. Kreps. 2013. “Political Parties at War.” American
Political Science Review 107(4):833–48.
Flores-Macías, Gustavo A., and Sarah E. Kreps. 2017. “Borrowing Support for War.” Journal of
Conflict Resolution 61(5):997–1020.
Foyle, Douglas C. 1999. Counting the Public In. New York: Columbia University Press.
Gadarian, Shana Kushner. 2010. “Foreign Policy at the Ballot Box.” Journal of Politics
72(4):1046–62.
Gelpi, Christopher. 1997. “Democratic Diversions.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 41(2):255–
82.
Gelpi, Christopher. 2010. “Performing on Cue?” The Formation of Public Opinion toward War.”
Journal of Conflict Resolution 54(1):88–116.
Gelpi, Christopher. 2017 “Democracies in Conflict.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 61(9):1925–
49.
Gelpi, Christopher, Peter D. Feaver and Jason Reifler. 2009. Paying the Human Costs of War.
Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.
42
Gelpi, Christopher, and Joseph M. Grieco. 2015. “Competency Costs in Foreign Affairs.”
American Journal of Political Science 59(2):440–56.
Gelpi, Christopher, Jason Reifler, and Peter Feaver. 2007. “Iraq the Vote.” Political Behavior
29(2):151–74.
Green, Donald P., Soo Yeon Kim, and David H. Yoon. 2001. “Dirty Pool.” International
Organization 55(2):441–68.
Grossman, Guy, Devorah Manekin, and Dan Miodownik. 2015. “The Political Legacies of
Combat.” International Organization 69(4):981–1009.
Guisinger, Alexandra. 2017. American Opinion on Trade. New York: Oxford University Press.
Guisinger, Alexandra, and Elizabeth N. Saunders. 2017. “Mapping the Boundaries of Elite
Cues.” International Studies Quarterly 61(2):425–41.
Hafner-Burton, Emilie M., Brad L. LeVeck, David G. Victor, and James H. Fowler. 2014.
“Decision Maker Preferences for International Legal Cooperation.” International
Organization 68(4):845–76.
Heffington, Colton, Brandon Beomseob Park, and Laron K Williams. Forthcoming. “The ‘Most
Important Problem’ Dataset (MIPD).” Conflict Management and Peace Science.
Herrmann, Richard K. 2017. “How Attachments to the Nation Shape Beliefs About the World.”
International Organization 71(S1):S61–84.
Herrmann, Richard K., and Jonathan W. Keller. 2004. “Beliefs, Values, and Strategic Choice.”
Journal of Politics 66(2):557–80.
Herrmann, Richard K., Philip E. Tetlock, and Penny S. Visser. 1999. “Mass Public Decisions to
Go to War: A Cognitive- Interactionist Framework.” American Political Science Review
93(3):553–73.
43
Holsti, Ole R. 2004. Public Opinion and American Foreign Policy. Ann Arbor: University of
Michigan Press.
Horowitz, Michael C., Allan C. Stam, and Cali M. Ellis. 2015. Why Leaders Fight. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Howell, William G., and Jon C. Pevehouse. 2007. While Dangers Gather. Princeton: Princeton
University Press.
Hurwitz, Jon, and Mark Peffley. 1987a. “How Are Foreign Policy Attitudes Structured? A
Hierarchical Model.” American Political Science Review 81(4):1099–1120.
Hurwitz, Jon, and Mark Peffley. 1987b. “The Means and Ends of Foreign Policy as Determinants
of Presidential Support.” American Journal of Political Science 31(2):236–58.
Jacobs, Lawrence R. and Benjamin I Page. 2005. “Who Influences U.S. Foreign Policy?”
American Political Science Review 99(1):107–23.
Jacobs, Lawrence R. and Robert Y. Shapiro. 2000. Politicians Don’t Pander. Chicago, IL: The
University of Chicago Press.
Johns, Robert, and Graeme AM Davies. 2012. “Democratic Peace or Clash of Civilizations?”
Journal of Politics 74(4):1038–52.
Johns, Robert, and Graeme AM Davies. 2014. “Coalitions of the Willing?” Journal of Peace
Research 51(6):767–81.
Kertzer, Joshua D., and Ryan Brutger. 2016. “Decomposing Audience Costs.” American Journal
of Political Science 60(1):234–49.
Kertzer, Joshua D., Kathleen Powers, Brian C. Rathbun, and Ravi Iyer. 2014. “Do Moral Values
Shape Foreign Policy Preferences?” Journal of Politics 76(3):825–40.
44
Kertzer, Joshua D., and Thomas Zeitzoff. 2017. “A Bottom-Up Theory of Public Opinion about
Foreign Policy.” American Journal of Political Science 61(3):543–58.
Lake, David A. “Powerful Pacifists.” American Political Science Review 86(1):24–37.
Leeds, Brett Ashley. 1999. “Domestic Political Institutions, Credible Commitments, and
International Cooperation.” American Journal of Political Science 43(4):979–1002.
Leeds, Brett Ashley. 2003. “Alliance Reliability in Times of War.” International Organization
57(4):801–827.
Levendusky, Matthew S., and Michael C. Horowitz. 2012. “When Backing Down Is the Right
Decision.” Journal of Politics 74(2):323–38.
Mansfield, Edward, Helen Milner and B. Peter Rosendorff. 2000. “Free to Trade.” American
Political Science Review 94(2):305–22.
Maoz, Zeev, and Bruce Russett. 1993. “Normative and Structural Causes of the Democratic
Peace, 1946–1986.” American Political Science Review 87(3):624–38.
McGillivray, Fiona, and Alastair Smith. 2008. Punishing the Prince. Princeton: Princeton
University Press.
Milner, Helen V. 1997. Interests, Institutions and Information. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Milner, Helen V., and Dustin Tingley. 2013. “Public Opinion and Foreign Aid.” International
Interactions 39(3):389–401.
Milner, Helen V., and Dustin Tingley. 2016. Sailing the Water’s Edge: The Domestic Politics of
American Foreign Policy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Mintz, Alex. 2004. “Foreign Policy Decision Making in Familiar and Unfamiliar Settings.”
Journal of Conflict Resolution 48(1):91–104.
45
Mintz, Alex, Nehemia Geva, Steven B. Redd and Amy Carnes. 1997. “The Effect of Dynamic
and Static Choice Sets on Political Decision Making.” American Political Science Review
91(3):553–66.
Moravcsik, Andrew. 1997. “Taking Preferences Seriously.” International Organization
51(4):513–53.
Morgan, T. Clifton, and Sally Howard Campbell. 1991. “Domestic Structure, Decisional
Constraints, and War.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 35(2):187–211.
Page, Benjamin I. 1994. “Democratic Responsiveness?” PS: Political Science and Politics
27(1):25–29.
Page, Benjamin I., and Richard A. Brody. 1972. “Policy Voting and the Electoral Process.”
American Political Science Review 66(3):979–95.
Page, Benjamin I., and Robert Y. Shapiro. 1983. “Effects of Public Opinion on Policy.”
American Political Science Review 77(1):175–90.
Popkin, Samuel L. 1991. The Reasoning Voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Potter, Phillip B.K. 2016. “Lame-Duck Foreign Policy.” Presidential Studies Quarterly
46(4):849–867.
Powlick, Philip J. 1991. “The Attitudinal Bases for Responsiveness to Public Opinion among
American Foreign Policy Officials.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 35(4):611–41.
Prather, Lauren. 2017. “Values at the Water’s Edge.” Working paper, University of California,
San Diego.
Press, Daryl G., Scott D. Sagan, and Benjamin A. Valentino. 2013. “Atomic Aversion.”
American Political Science Review 107(1):188–206.
Putnam, Robert D. 1988. “Diplomacy and Domestic Politics.” International Organization
46
42(3):427–60.
Quek, Kai. 2016. “Are Costly Signals More Credible?” Journal of Politics 78(3):925–40
Reifler, Jason, Thomas J. Scotto, and Harold D. Clarke. 2011. “Foreign Policy Beliefs in
Contemporary Britain: Structure and Relevance.” International Studies Quarterly
55(1):245–66.
Reiter, Dan, and Allan C. Stam. 2002. Democracies at War. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Risse-Kappen, Thomas. 1991. “Public Opinion, Domestic Structure, and Foreign Policy in
Liberal Democracies.” World Politics 43(4):479–512.
Russett, Bruce M. 1990. Controlling the Sword. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Russett, Bruce and John R. Oneal. 2001. Triangulating Peace. New York: Norton
Saunders, Elizabeth N. 2011. Leaders at War. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Saunders, Elizabeth N. 2015. “War and the Inner Circle: Democratic Elites and the Politics of
Using Force.” Security Studies 24(3):466–501.
Schultz, Kenneth. 2001. “Looking for Audience Costs.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 45(1):32–
60.
Simmons, Beth A. 2009. Mobilizing for Human Rights. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Snyder, Jack, and Erica D. Borghard. 2011. “The Cost of Empty Threats.” American Political
Science Review 105(3):437–56.
Spilker, Gabriele, Thomas Bernauer, and Víctor Umaña. 2016. “Selecting Partner Countries for
Preferential Trade Agreements.” International Studies Quarterly 60(4):706–18.
Stein, Rachel M. 2015. “War and Revenge: Explaining Conflict Initiation by Democracies.”
American Political Science Review 109(3):556–73.
47
Stimson, James A., Michael B. Mackuen, and Robert S. Erikson. 1995. “Dynamic
Representation.” American Political Science Review 89(3):543–65.
Tanaka, Seiki, Atsushi Tago, and Kristian S. Gleditsch. 2017. “Seeing the Lexus for the Olive
Trees?” International Interactions 43(3):375–96.
Tingley, Dustin. 2017. “Rising Power on the Mind.” International Organization 71(S1):S165–
88.
Trager, Robert F., and Lynn Vavreck. 2011. “The Political Costs of Crisis Bargaining.”
American Journal of Political Science 55(3):526–45.
Wlezien, Christopher. 2005. “On the Salience of Political Issues.Electoral Studies 24(4):555–
79.