fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 11 September 2020
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02232
Edited by:
Peter L. Fisher,
University of Liverpool,
United Kingdom
Reviewed by:
Tobias Kube,
Harvard Medical School,
United States
Brooke Schneider,
University Medical Center
Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
*Correspondence:
Bjørn Ingulfsvann Hagen
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Psychology for Clinical Settings,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 28 April 2020
Accepted: 10 August 2020
Published: 11 September 2020
Citation:
Hagen BI, Landrø NI, Lau B,
Koster EHW and Stubberud J (2020)
Predictors of Long-Term Improvement
Following Cognitive Remediation in a
Sample With Elevated Depressive
Symptoms. Front. Psychol. 11:2232.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02232
Predictors of Long-Term
Improvement Following Cognitive
Remediation in a Sample With
Elevated Depressive Symptoms
Bjørn Ingulfsvann Hagen
1
*
, Nils Inge Landrø
2
, Bjørn Lau
2
, Ernst H. W. Koster
3
and
Jan Stubberud
1,2
1
Department of Research, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway,
2
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo,
Oslo, Norway,
3
Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Objective: Cognitive remediation (CR) techniques (interventions to enhance cognitive
functioning) have proven moderately effective in improving cognition and daily
functioning in major depressive disorder (MDD). However, baseline predictors of
treatment response are lacking. The present study aimed to identify factors influencing
long-term CR outcomes in a sample with current or previous, mild or moderate MDD
and with self-reported cognitive deficits.
Methods: Forty-two completers of group-based CR (strategy learning or drill-and-
practice), were pooled into one sample. Based on change scores from baseline to
6-month follow-up, participants were categorized as “improvers” or “non-improvers”
using reliable change index calculations. Measures included a questionnaire of everyday
executive functioning and a neuropsychological test of attention. Finally, improvers and
non-improvers were compared in terms of various sociodemographic, psychological,
illness-related, and neuropsychological baseline variables.
Results: Seventeen participants improved reliably in everyday executive functioning,
and fourteen demonstrated a reliable improvement in attention. No statistically significant
differences emerged between improvers and non-improvers.
Conclusion: No major predictors of CR were identified. Importantly, the current findings
are insufficient to guide clinical decision-making. Large-scale studies with a priori
hypotheses are needed to make advances in the future.
Keywords: depression, cognitive remediation, treatment predictors, executive functions, attention
INTRODUCTION
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is characterized by deficits in cognitive functions, including
attention, memory, and executive functions (EF) (Snyder, 2012; Ahern and Semkovska, 2017).
However, the heterogeneity of the cognitive profile in MDD appears to be large, with distinct
neurocognitive subgroups (Pu et al., 2018). For those experiencing cognitive difficulties, deficits
often persist into remission (Rock et al., 2014) and have a deleterious effect on everyday
functioning (Baune et al., 2010). Cognitive deficits, particularly in EF, are additionally associated
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 2
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
with unfavorable depression outcomes, such as impaired
long-term recovery (Vicent-Gil et al., 2018) and reduced
antidepressant medication effectiveness (Groves et al., 2018).
Hence, cognitive functioning represents a potential treatment
target in MDD (Kaser et al., 2017).
Nonetheless, both antidepressant medication and cognitive–
behavioral therapy (CBT) show limited effectiveness in alleviating
cognitive deficits (Porter et al., 2016; Shilyansky et al.,
2016). Cognitive remediation (CR) interventions specifically
targeting cognitive dysfunction intended to produce lasting
improvements in everyday functioning are thus emerging.
Although the heterogeneity of treatment approaches labeled
“CR is large (Motter et al., 2016), these interventions can be
divided into either bottom-up drill-and-practice approaches or
top-down approaches focusing on strategy learning. Bottom-up
approaches typically consist of computerized cognitive training
(CCT) tasks intended to improve basic cognitive processes
through the process of neuroplasticity (Motter et al., 2016). In
contrast, top-down approaches consist of learning compensatory
strategies for wide appliance in daily living, to compensate for
the cognitive difficulties. Findings indicate moderate effectiveness
in improving cognition and everyday functioning following CR,
but there is a paucity of evidence for improved EF or on its long-
term effects (for a meta-analysis, see Motter et al., 2016). In this
context, it has been argued that the substantial heterogeneity in
the cognitive profile of MDD may influence the effectiveness of
CR (Koster et al., 2017).
The lack of factors associated with successful treatment
outcomes may be a barrier to improving CR effectiveness
(Motter et al., 2016; Koster et al., 2017). The identification of
pretreatment predictors could improve efficacy by facilitating
individualized clinical decision-making. Moreover, it may be
helpful for the development of future treatments by providing
insight into the mechanisms of CR interventions (Koster et al.,
2017). The investigation of CR predictors and moderators
in MDD is scarce, but a meta-analysis covering a range of
CCT interventions found decreased treatment effectiveness with
increased age (Motter et al., 2016). Additional variables, such
as gender and receiving concurrent treatment (antidepressant
medication or psychotherapy), did not significantly influence
outcomes. In a recent study dedicated to identifying the
predictors in a CR intervention consisting of both CCT and
strategy learning, Listunova et al. (2020) observed a shorter
duration of illness to be the only factor associated with
improvement on a neuropsychological measure of attention in
a partially remitted MDD sample. Several sociodemographic,
neurocognitive, psychopathological, and training-specific factors
thus failed to predict outcomes. However, the study was
limited by its exploratory approach, modest sample size, and
exclusive focus on the attention domain (Listunova et al.,
2020). Interestingly, CR findings diverge from psychotherapy
research in MDD, where illness characteristics such as greater
depression symptom severity, younger age at onset, and more
previous episodes have all been associated with poorer responses
in relation to depressive symptom alleviation (Hamilton and
Dobson, 2002). Moreover, in schizophrenia research, where
predictors and moderators of CR effectiveness have been
more frequently studied, most of the factors reviewed fail to
significantly influence CR treatment response (Reser et al.,
2019; Seccomandi et al., 2020). However, in a selection
of studies, better baseline performance in several cognitive
domains predicted both cognitive and functional improvement,
while increased chronicity and severity of schizophrenia has
been associated with worse CR outcomes (Medalia and
Richardson, 2005; Kurtz et al., 2009; Vita et al., 2013;
Lindenmayer et al., 2017).
The main aim of the present study was to explore whether
a selection of sociodemographic, neuropsychological, illness-
related, and psychological variables could predict long-term
CR outcomes in a sample with current or previous mild or
moderate MDD. Data were collected as part of a single-blind
randomized controlled trial (RCT) comparing the effectiveness
of a strategy-based CR approach, Goal Management Training
(GMT), with drill-and-practice CCT, in improving EF (Hagen
et al., 2020). Both groups improved on measures of EF in daily
life, neuropsychological tests of EF and attention, and depression
symptom severity following CR. That is, no significant differences
emerged between groups in the original study, although within-
group changes in everyday EF and depression symptom severity
were only significant following GMT. Owing to the limited
number of previous studies examining predictors of CR in MDD,
the present study applied an exploratory approach with no
a priori hypothesis.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
The original RCT was preregistered at clinicaltrials.gov with the
identifier NCT03338413, and the study protocol was approved by
the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics,
South-Eastern Norway (2017/666). The study was conducted
following the World Medical Association’s Declaration of
Helsinki, and all participants gave their written informed consent.
For more detailed information on the methodological approach
of the original RCT study, see Hagen et al. (2020).
Participants
The sample (n = 63) included participants diagnosed with mild
or moderate MDD according to International Classification
of Diseases 10th Revision (ICD-10) criteria (World Health
Organization, 2004), either as a primary or as a secondary
diagnosis. All participants had undergone a diagnostic
evaluation and completed treatment at the Return-to-Work
clinic at Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital within 2 years
before inclusion. The Return-to-Work clinic offers short-
term outpatient psychotherapeutic treatment to patients
with mental health issues of mild to moderate severity, at
risk of receiving sick leave because of mental illness. In
addition, to be included, participants had to be between 18
and 60 years of age and to have self-reported everyday EF
deficits (e.g., difficulties with memory, organizing/planning,
emotional regulation, and/or concentration) in a custom-
made telephone interview. All participants were additionally
asked to confirm that depression symptoms represent a
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 3
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
major mental health complaint. Exclusion criteria included
comorbid neurological conditions, ongoing alcohol or substance
abuse, and severe cognitive problems or mental disorders
(psychotic disorders and severe personality disorders).
No cutoff scores were specified for current depressive
symptom severity. There were no restrictions on participants
concerning additional concurrent psychotherapeutic or
antidepressant treatment.
Study Design and Blinding
All participants completed a baseline assessment (T1) before
being randomly assigned to nine sessions of either GMT
or CCT, using computer-generated simple randomization.
Participants were reassessed immediately following treatment
completion (T2) and at a 6-month follow-up (T3). The
assessments consisted of neuropsychological tests and self-
report rating scales. Assessments were not blind, because
the person responsible for data collection also acted as a
therapist in both interventions. To compensate, an external
assessor performed a limited set of blind T3 assessments
(n = 5) for comparison. However, the study was single-
blind because participants were not informed whether they
had been allocated to the condition considered to be the
active treatment.
Cognitive Remediation Interventions
Goal Management Training
GMT is a manual-based CR intervention to improve everyday
EF (Levine et al., 2011). A central element of GMT is to learn
and internalize strategies for wide application in daily living,
promoting goal-directed behavior through increased executive
control and improved problem-solving capacities. Strategies
consist of a self-instruction to stop ongoing behavior, check
the current content of working memory, state and define
goals, apply a systematic approach to problem-solving, and
monitor performance.
In the present study, the Norwegian translation of the
standard GMT protocol was employed (Stubberud et al.,
2013; Tornås et al., 2016). A clinical psychologist and a
neuropsychologist delivered GMT in groups of five to seven
participants in 9 weekly 2 h sessions. In-class exercises included
practicing the use of the compensatory strategies (e.g., practice a
systematic approach to problem-solving by arranging a fictitious
wedding party). Mindfulness exercises (Kabat-Zinn, 1990),
intended to enhance attentional control, were also practiced in
class. Sessions emphasized group discussions addressing personal
examples of dysexecutive behavior. Between-session assignments
included monitoring EF-related errors, mindfulness exercises,
and the application of learned strategies in daily life (Table 1).
Computerized Cognitive Training
The CCT consisted of seven exercises from the BrainHQ
platform. Repetition is the hallmark feature of CCT, and
neuroplasticity is its theoretical foundation (Siegle et al., 2007).
Cognitive improvements, including EF, have been identified
using BrainHQ or similar exercises (Morimoto et al., 2014;
Lewandowski et al., 2017), and these studies established the
empirical basis for the selection of exercises. In addition, exercise
selection was based on the provider’s description
1
.
The CCT consisted of nine twice-weekly 1 h sessions by groups
of three participants. Exercises targeted attention, memory,
processing speed, and EF. To ensure appropriate levels of mastery
and frustration, the platform adapted difficulty levels to the
individual participants’ performance, keeping the success rate
at 80% throughout. A clinical psychologist acted as a therapist
and gave participants positive feedback on their efforts. The first
session included psychoeducation, with the therapist introducing
the concept of neuroplasticity, typical cognitive deficits in
depression, and the importance of cognitive processes in different
everyday situations. Participants had online access to the training
platform and were encouraged to practice for at least 30 min
between each session (Table 1).
Completer Sample
Participants had to attend a minimum of six training sessions
and complete the 6-month follow-up assessment (T3) to be
included in the completer sample. Forty-two completers from
both groups were pooled into one sample. The pooling of
participants receiving different treatments was done to increase
the sample size (Figure 1).
Outcome Measures
The Global Executive Composite (GEC) from the Behavior
Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Adult version (BRIEF-
A) (Roth et al., 2005) was applied as an outcome of self-reported
everyday EF. The BRIEF-A GEC consists of 70 items and nine
non-overlapping subscales (Inhibit, Self-Monitor, Plan/Organize,
Shift, Initiate, Task Monitor, Emotional Control, Working
Memory, Organization of Materials), tapping the frequency of
everyday dysexecutive behavior (item range 1–3, total range 70–
210). The psychometric properties of the GEC are acceptable,
with a 1-month test–retest reliability of 0.94 and a Cronbachs
alpha of 0.96 (Roth et al., 2005).
The Conners Continuous Performance Test Third edition
(CPT-3) (Conners, 2015) was applied as a neuropsychological
measure of attention. The CPT-3 is a 14 min go/no-go
test of visual attentiveness, response inhibition, and sustained
attention. The number of commission errors (Commissions;
response to “no-go” targets: attentiveness and inhibition) and
hit reaction time standard deviation (HRT SD: response
consistency/sustained attention) subscales were included as
outcome measures. The corrected test–retest reliabilities (1–5
weeks) of the included subscales are 0.68 (HRT SD) and 0.85
(Commissions) (Conners, 2015). A neuropsychological measure
of attention was selected as an outcome to facilitate comparison
with the only previous study that we know of that investigated the
predictors of CR response in MDD (Listunova et al., 2020).
Predictors of Treatment Effect
Sociodemographic Factors
Sociodemographic factors included age, gender, years of
education, and employment status, all self-reported in a
1
https://www.brainhq.com
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 4
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
TABLE 1 | GMT sessions, CCT tasks, and objectives.
GMT session Objective CCT task Objective
The present and the absent mind Tracking absentmindedness/Practice mindfulness techniques Double decision Attention
Inattentive Errors Condition for, and consequences of, absentmindedness Divided attention Attention/Inhibition
The Automatic Pilot How automatic behavior leads to inappropriate responding Target tracker Attention
Stop the automatic pilot Make a habit of stopping/Bring attention to the present Syllable stacks Working memory
The mental blackboard Check the content of working memory Scene crasher Working memory
State your goal State goals to facilitate goal maintenance Face-to-Face Processing Speed/ Social Cognition
Making decisions Goal conflict in decision-making/Making to-do lists Sound sweeps Processing speed
Splitting tasks into subtasks Split overwhelming tasks into subtasks/Step-by-step approach
Checking (STOP!) Adapting behavior to situational change/Summary of training
GMT, Goal Management Training; CCT, computerized cognitive training.
custom-made interview. Employment status was transformed
into a dichotomous variable with the categories “full-time
employment/full-time student and “other” (including
“part-time employment/part-time student, “sick leave, and
“looking for a job”).
Illness-Related Factors
Illness-related factors included current depressive symptom
severity in addition to the self-reported number of previous
depressive episodes, age of onset, duration of illness, and current
antidepressant medication use. Depressive symptom severity was
assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al.,
1961), which has satisfactory internal consistency (Beck et al.,
1988). The remaining illness-related factors were self-reported
during a custom-made interview. The number of previous
episodes was transformed into a dichotomous variable based on
the categories of “one episode and “more than one episode, as
this was considered a theoretically meaningful subdivision of the
highly skewed original continuous variable.
Psychological Factors
Psychological factors included overall psychological distress and
a tendency to ruminate. The Clinical Outcomes in Routine
Evaluation Outcome Measure (CORE-OM) (Barkham et al.,
2001) was applied as a measure of overall psychological distress.
The CORE-OM clinical score was calculated as a mean of
completed items multiplied by 10 (range: 0–40). Rumination was
assessed using the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) (Treynor
et al., 2003). Both questionnaires have acceptable internal
consistency (Barkham et al., 2001; Treynor et al., 2003).
Neuropsychological Factors
Neuropsychological factors included estimated IQ, assessed with
the two-subtest form (Matrix reasoning; Vocabulary) of the
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler,
1999). Performance on the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function
System (D-KEFS) (Delis et al., 2001) Trail-Making Test was
applied as a measure of processing speed (condition 2) and
EF/shifting (condition 4). Memory was assessed using the
California Verbal Learning Test–Second edition Short form
(CVLT-II SF) (Delis et al., 2000). The digit span forward
(attention span) and digit span backward (working memory)
subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth edition
(WAIS-IV) (Wechsler, 2014), were also employed.
Other Factors
Received intervention (GMT or CCT) was included as a training-
specific factor. In addition, baseline performance scores on the
outcome variables were included as predictors.
Statistical Analysis
Calculation of Reliable Change Index
The reliable change index (RCI) (Jacobson and Truax, 1991)
was calculated for everyday EF (BRIEF-A GEC) and the
neuropsychological measure of attention (CPT-3: Commissions,
HRT SD). The RCI analysis is a statistical approach to
identify individuals with statistically reliable improvement,
given the scale reliability. Thus, the approach is sensitive to
individual participant improvements potentially lost in group-
level statistical analysis (Jacobson and Truax, 1991). To calculate
the RCI, a change in individual raw score (BRIEF-A) or T-score
(CPT-3) between T1 (X
1
) and T3 (X
2
) was divided by the
standard error of the difference (SE
diff
) using the formula:
RCI = (X
2
X
1
)/SE
diff
SE
diff
was derived from the standard error of measurement (S
E
),
calculated using the test–retest reliability (r
xx
) of the instrument,
and the standard deviation (SD) using the following formulas:
S
E
= S
p
(1 r
xx
) and SE
diff
=
p
2 (S
E
)
2
An RCI smaller than –1.96 (because of measurement
direction) was required to be considered as a reliable
improvement. A change surpassing the ±1.96 threshold
occurs by chance in only 5% of cases. Information on test–retest
reliability and standard deviation was collected from the test
manuals (Roth et al., 2005; Conners, 2015). The decision to
calculate change scores from baseline (T1) to the 6-month
follow-up (T3) was because long-term outcomes were regarded
as most clinically relevant. For the CPT-3, a reliable improvement
on one of the two subscales (Commissions or HRT SD) was
required to count as an improvement. Finally, participants
improving reliably on one subscale were not included if there
was a reliable deterioration on the other.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 5
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
FIGURE 1 | CONSORT diagram (Schulz et al., 2010).
Comparison of Improvers and Non-improvers
For each of the two outcome measures, participants were
categorized as either “improvers or “non-improvers based
on their RCI score. Improvers were compared with non-
improvers in the pooled completer samples using the non-
parametric Mann–Whitney U test and chi-square test, for
pairs of continuous and dichotomous variables, respectively.
In addition, the T3 results on the outcome measures were
compared between assessors (blind/non-blind), and T1 results
between completers and non-completers, using the Mann–
Whitney U test. All tests were two-tailed, and to partially
account for multiple testing, the significance level was set
to 0.01. Values between 0.01 and 0.05 were interpreted
as trends. SPSS version 24.0 for Windows was applied
for all analyses.
RESULTS
The completers (n = 42) had a median age of 41 years (range = 28–
59) and a median of 15 years of education (range = 9–
18). The majority were female (79.1%), and 76.7% did not
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 6
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
TABLE 2 | Comorbid ICD-10 diagnoses in the completer sample.
Disorder Frequency
Generalized anxiety disorder (F 41.1) 6
Panic disorder (F 41.0) 3
Post-traumatic stress disorder (F 43.1) 1
Agoraphobia (F 40.0) 1
Social phobia (F 40.1) 1
Anxiety disorder, unspecified (F 41.9) 1
Nonorganic insomnia (F 51.0) 2
Mental and behavioral disorders due to
psychoactive substance use (F 10–F 19)
2
Number of participants with comorbidities, n = 14. Multiple comorbid diagnoses
are possible for each participant. ICD-10 = International Classification of Diseases
10th Revision.
currently use antidepressant medication. Furthermore, their
average depression symptom severity was in the mild range (BDI:
median = 17.0, range = 4.0–34.0) (Beck et al., 1988). Fourteen
completers had been diagnosed with a comorbid ICD-10 mental
or behavioral disorder (Table 2).
The sample reported substantial executive dysfunction in daily
living (BRIEF-A GEC T-score: median = 64, range = 44–80)
but performed in the normal range for the included CPT-
3 subscales at baseline (Commissions T-score: median = 48,
range = 35–73; HRT SD T-score: median = 44, range = 33–
75). At follow-up, completers reported overall fewer EF
deficits in daily life (BRIEF-A GEC T-score: median = 59.5,
range = 36–78) and performed better on the measure of attention
(CPT-3: Commissions T-score, median = 44, range = 25–
71; HRT SD T-score, median = 41, range = 31–56). No
statistically significant differences emerged for any of the
outcome measures at follow-up between the blinded and non-
blinded assessors. Finally, the completers were not significantly
different from the non-completers (n = 21) on any of the
included variables.
Comparison of Everyday Executive
Functioning of Improvers and
Non-improvers
Seventeen participants (40.5%) were identified to improve
reliably on the BRIEF-A GEC between T1 and T3. For
participants to surpass the critical value for improvement,
a 13-point reduction in BRIEF-A GEC raw score was
required. The mean BRIEF-A GEC raw score of improvers
at follow-up was 107.9 (SD = 17.9). No statistically significant
differences emerged between improvers and non-improvers
for any of the predictors. However, improvers had a
higher estimated IQ than non-improvers at trend level
(p = 0.044) (Table 3).
Comparison of Attention by Improvers
and Non-improvers
Fourteen participants (33.3%) were identified as improvers on
the measure of attention. Twelve participants improved on the
Commissions subscale and four on the HRT SD subscale, while
TABLE 3 | Characteristics of improvers and non-improvers in everyday EF.
Variable Improvers Non-improvers p-value
(n = 17) (n = 25)
Categorical variables
Gender (female) 14 19 0.622
Work status (full-time) 11 18 0.616
Antidepressant use (yes) 2 8 0.131
Dep. ep. (1 ep.) 5 9 0.754
Received intervention (GMT) 10 13 0.663
Continuous variables Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Age (years) 44.9 (8.7) 40.8 (8.5) 0.098
Years of education 15.5 (2.0) 14.4 (2.1) 0.071
Age of onset (years) 32.7 (12.9) 26.0 (10.3) 0.087
Illness duration (years) 12.2 (10.4) 14.8 (9.7) 0.317
WASI (IQ estimate) 115.3 (6.7) 108.8 (12.1) 0.044
TMT 2 (seconds) 30.8 (10.2) 31.4 (9.7) 0.488
TMT 4 (seconds) 71.2 (21.6) 84.1 (34.6) 0.109
CVLT-II SF Long delay 8.4 (1.2) 8.3 (0.8) 0.299
WAIS-IV DS Forward 9.7 (2.4) 9.1 (1.5) 0.539
WAIS-IV DS Backward 10.0 (2.3) 9.8 (1.8) 0.815
BDI 19.0 (6.7) 15.2 (7.1) 0.063
RRS 56.3 (10.5) 55.0 (12.2) 0.710
CORE-OM 14.7 (5.5) 13.4 (6.0) 0.522
CPT-3 Commissions (T-score) 50.2 (11.3) 51.5 (8.4) 0.336
CPT-3 HRT SD (T-score) 46.6 (9.8) 44.8 (9.8) 0.293
BRIEF-A GEC 137.3 (17.9) 128.6 (15.2) 0.124
SD, standard deviation, Dep., depression; ep., episode; GMT, Goal Management
Training; WASI, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; TMT, Trail-Making
Test; CVLT-II SF, California Verbal Learning Test–Second edition Short form;
WAIS-IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth edition; DS, digit span; BDI,
Beck Depression Inventory; RRS, Ruminative Response Scale; CORE-OM, Clinical
Outcomes in Routine Evaluation Outcome Measure; CPT, Conners Continuous
Performance Test; HRT, hit reaction time; BRIEF-A GEC, Behavior Rating Inventory
of Executive Function Adult version Global Executive Composite; EF, executive
functions. All scores reported are raw scores, except when otherwise specified.
Higher scores indicate greater impairment, except for WASI, CVLT-II, and WAIS-IV.
two participants improved on both subscales. For participants to
surpass the critical value for improvement, a change in T-score
of 10 (Commissions) or 13 (HRT SD) was required. At follow-
up, the mean T-score for the improver group (n = 14) was 42.2
(SD = 6.5) on the Commission subscale and 39.9 (SD = 7.5)
on the HRT SD subscale. No statistically significant differences
emerged between improvers and non-improvers. However, at
trend level, fewer of the improvers compared with the non-
improvers (p = 0.011) had experienced only one previous
depressive episode (Table 4). The mean number of self-reported
previous depressive episodes was 4.8 (SD = 3.3) for improvers
and 3.4 (SD = 3.2) for non-improvers (values > 10 were
recoded > 10 = 10).
Overlap Between Improvers Across
Outcomes
Fifteen participants (35.7%) improved on neither measure, and
four participants (9.5%) improved reliably on both the BRIEF-A
GEC and the CPT-3.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 7
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
TABLE 4 | Characteristics of improvers and non-improvers in attention.
Variable Improvers Non-improvers p-value
(n = 14) (n = 28)
Categorical variables
Gender (female) 11 22 0.999
Work status (full-time) 10 19 0.813
Antidepressant use (yes) 1 9 0.073
Dep. ep. (1 ep.) 1 14 0.011
Received intervention (GMT) 7 16 0.661
Continuous variables Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Age (years) 42.9 (9.4) 42.3 (8.5) 0.915
Years of education 15.1 (2.1) 14.8 (2.1) 0.539
Age of onset (years) 29.3 (13.6) 28.4 (11.0) 0.957
Illness duration (years) 13.6 (9.3) 13.9 (10.4) 0.947
WASI (IQ-estimate) 114.9 (6.4) 109.6 (12.0) 0.194
TMT 2 (seconds) 30.3 (7.6) 31.5 (10.8) 0.904
TMT 4 (seconds) 79.1 (21.4) 79.2 (34.3) 0.566
CVLT-II SF Long delay 8.3 (0.9) 8.3 (1.0) 0.801
WAIS-IV DS Forward 10.1 (2.1) 9.0 (1.7) 0.092
WAIS-IV DS Backward 10.5 (1.8) 9.6 (2.0) 0.151
BDI 18.8 (6.5) 15.7 (7.3) 0.169
RRS 58.4 (10.7) 54.1 (11.7) 0.279
CORE-OM 16.5 (4.9) 12.6 (5.9) 0.056
CPT-3 Commissions (T-score) 54.8 (9.3) 49.0 (9.3) 0.050
CPT-3 HRT SD (T-score) 49.5 (13.3) 43.5 (6.7) 0.224
BRIEF-A GEC 137.6 (14.5) 129.4 (17.3) 0.157
SD, standard deviation; Dep., depression; ep., episode; GMT, Goal Management
Training; WASI, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; TMT, Trail-Making
Test; CVLT-II SF, California Verbal Learning Test–Second edition Short form;
WAIS-IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth edition; DS, digit span; BDI,
Beck Depression Inventory; RRS, Ruminative Response Scale; CORE-OM, Clinical
Outcomes in Routine Evaluation Outcome Measure; CPT, Conners’ Continuous
Performance Test; HRT, hit reaction time; BRIEF-A GEC, Behavior Rating Inventory
of Executive Function Adult version Global Executive Composite. All scores
reported are raw scores, except when otherwise specified. Higher scores indicate
greater impairment, except for WASI, CVLT-II, and WAIS-IV.
DISCUSSION
The present study aimed to identify factors predicting long-term
treatment outcomes following CR in an MDD sample. None of
the variables emerged as major predictors of change in either
everyday EF or attention. The lack of factors associated with CR
improvement is generally consistent with previous research on
MDD (Motter et al., 2016; Listunova et al., 2020).
Even though none of the illness-related factors emerged
as major predictors, surprisingly, a reliable improvement in
attention was associated at trend level with having experienced
more than one previous depressive episode. Recurrence of
episodes arguably indicates greater illness severity and chronicity,
previously associated with reduced CR effectiveness for both
MDD (Listunova et al., 2020) and schizophrenia (Medalia and
Richardson, 2005; Vita et al., 2013; Lindenmayer et al., 2017).
This result thus diverges from a selection of previous findings.
However, contrary to conclusions from systematic reviews in
MDD (Rock et al., 2014; Ahern and Semkovska, 2017), the
present sample did not display objective attention deficits at
baseline. Additionally, previous research has suggested distinct
neurocognitive subgroups for MDD, with a majority showing
near-normative performance on neuropsychological tests (Pu
et al., 2018). Participants in the Return-to-Work program
report less overall illness severity and are more likely to hold
a job compared with other outpatients (Victor et al., 2016).
In addition, IQ estimates were above average in the present
sample. Such sample characteristics could have contributed
to normal performance on the cognitive measures (Elgamal
et al., 2010; Venezia et al., 2018). Furthermore, owing to the
weak correlations between self-reported and neuropsychological
measures of cognition in MDD, the inclusion based on subjective
deficits may have also resulted in a subgroup of cognitively
unimpaired participants (Petersen et al., 2019). Notably, at
baseline, the non-improver group performed even better than
improvers on the outcome measure of attention and may as
such represent a part of the sample without actual attention
deficits. The non-improvers were thus less likely to surpass the
threshold for improvement, because their baseline left little room
for further gains. Indeed, not accounting for individual difference
in baseline performance is a limitation of the RCI approach
(Duff, 2012). Moreover, although overall results are mixed, some
previous findings indicate that MDD recurrence is related to
impaired performance on measures of cognition (Hasselbalch
et al., 2011), and this could potentially explain why recurrent
episodes were associated with attention improvement in our
sample when members of this group had the opportunity to
improve their attention as a result of the interventions.
The present study failed to replicate the single significant
finding from a previous meta-analysis investigating moderators
of CR outcomes in MDD, namely, that treatment effectiveness
decreases with increasing age (Motter et al., 2016). Excluding
participants above the age of 60 years restricted the age range and
reduced the sample variability of the present study, potentially
limiting the prospect of obtaining significant results. Nonetheless,
the overall available evidence does not indicate that age is a
reliable predictor of CR outcomes (Reser et al., 2019; Listunova
et al., 2020; Seccomandi et al., 2020). However, in accordance
with previous research on MDD (Motter et al., 2016; Listunova
et al., 2020), neither gender nor receiving concurrent treatments
predicted improvements in cognition or everyday EF, with the
latter indicating no additive effect on cognition of combining CR
with antidepressant medication.
Cognitive performance at baseline did not predict outcomes
for attention or EF in our sample. This finding is contrary to
a selection of findings regarding schizophrenia (Reser et al.,
2019) but in accordance with MDD research (Listunova et al.,
2020). One notable exception was that higher IQ estimates
were associated with improvement in everyday EF at the trend
level. Theoretically, a greater general ability may contribute to
reaching ones potential for applying learned strategies in daily
living, thereby increasing CR effectiveness (Velligan et al., 2006).
Hence, the role of IQ in CR should be further investigated
in future studies.
Delivering CR therapies to patients with MDD hinges on the
theoretical assumption that cognitive deficits impair everyday
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 8
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
functioning and act as risk factors for depressive symptoms.
Thus, it is striking that cognitive performance at baseline
(i.e., the degree of the cognitive deficits) lacks support as a
moderator of CR outcome (Koster et al., 2017). No established
neuropsychological profile exists for MDD (Marazziti et al.,
2010), and this heterogeneity could limit the chance of detecting
reliable pretreatment cognitive predictors. Furthermore, the
relationship of cognitive factors with outcomes may be non-
linear, exerting different influences at different levels of each
variable. To illustrate, higher baseline cognitive performance may
be conceptualized both as facilitating CR gains and as restricting
improvement potential (Twamley et al., 2011; Vita et al., 2013).
Rumination is proposed to have a bidirectional relationship
with EF (Davis and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Philippot and
Brutoux, 2008), and in CCT interventions specifically addressing
EF processes (i.e., cognitive control training), rumination has
been found to mediate depressive symptom outcomes (Quinn
et al., 2014). To our knowledge, no previous study has
investigated whether baseline rumination predicts CR outcomes
in cognition or functioning. Our findings suggest that baseline
rumination is not a major predictor of improvements in attention
or everyday EF. However, important subcomponents of the
rumination construct have been identified (Treynor et al., 2003)
but were not presently investigated.
Pooling participants who received CR interventions that
differed in content and theoretical foundation was necessary to
obtain an acceptable sample size. This may have obscured the
effect of predictors on each treatment. Nevertheless, the number
of improvers was similar across interventions for both outcome
measures in the present study; moreover, a previous meta-
analysis of schizophrenia has indicated that different approaches
produce similar overall effects on measures of cognition (Wykes
et al., 2011), suggesting commonalities between treatments.
Clinical Implications and Future
Directions
The percentage of improvers in the present study (33.3–
40.5%) and previous studies (34.2%) (Listunova et al., 2020)
indicates the potential to increase CR effectiveness in MDD. The
heterogeneity of cognitive deficits in depression suggests that
individualized interventions may be required, and understanding
why participants achieve different outcomes represents a critical
hurdle to individualizing CR. However, identifying easily
available major predictors of treatment outcomes has proven to
be a challenge, and current findings are insufficient to guide
clinical decision-making. Moreover, no consistent barriers to
improvement have been identified to date, so these findings
suggest that MDD patients have the potential to improve
following CR, regardless of their baseline characteristics. For
advances in the field, large-scale and fine-grained investigations
with a priori hypotheses are required (Reser et al., 2019;
Seccomandi et al., 2020). In addition, as has been suggested
for schizophrenia, we may need to go beyond generic
demographic and clinical factors to predict CR outcomes reliably
(Reser et al., 2019).
The present study focused on identifying baseline predictors
that could be easily disseminated into clinical practice. Hence, it
did not include several factors identified as potential mediators
or moderators of CR in MDD or schizophrenia, such as
the number of training sessions (Buonocore et al., 2017),
motivation/engagement with training (Medalia and Richardson,
2005; Siegle et al., 2014), therapist characteristics (e.g., clinical
experience) (Medalia and Richardson, 2005), and patient–
therapist working alliance (Huddy et al., 2012), all candidates for
further investigation.
Strengths and Limitations
The study was based on data from a single-blind RCT, applied
a multimodal selection of outcome measures, and used a
stringent approach to define improvement. In addition, the study
attempted to extend on previous research by applying a long
follow-up period as an endpoint, aiming to identify predictors of
durable change following CR. However, the following limitations
should be considered when interpreting the above findings.
No a priori hypotheses were generated, and the analyses were
exploratory, calling for caution in the interpretation of results.
Another notable limitation was the modest sample size, reducing
statistical power and increasing the risk of type II errors.
Furthermore, multiple testing inflated the risk of type I errors,
even if partially accounted for by lowering the significance level.
The neuropsychological measure of attention was not
corrected for practice effects (Chelune et al., 1993). Although
the overall practice effects on the CPT-3 are reported to be
small-to-moderate across the included subscales [T = 2.9 for
Commissions; T = 0.2 for HRT SD; Conners, 2015], correcting
for these would still provide reliable improvement in attention
that is very hard to achieve for a substantial proportion of
the sample, given the conservative RCI threshold and baseline
performance in the normal range. Moreover, the lack of an
adequate sample for comparison (i.e., non-intervention control
or comparable norm population) restricted the advantages of
applying more sophisticated statistical approaches to overcome
some of the above issues.
A selection of variables was transformed into dichotomous
categories, which may result in a loss of information and power
(MacCallum et al., 2002). Notably, this included the “number
of previous episodes variable, found to differ between groups
at trend level. The follow-up assessments lacked blinding for
most of the sample, increasing the risk of biased responding.
However, results from a small number of blind assessments were
not significantly different from those of non-blind assessments.
Finally, the illness-related variables (e.g., age of onset, number of
episodes) were self-reported and thus more susceptible to bias,
including memory biases.
CONCLUSION
In the present study, no major predictors of long-term
improvement in attention or executive functioning following CR
emerged. The results are consistent with previous research, which
mostly failed to identify predictors of CR treatment. Importantly,
the current findings are insufficient to guide clinical decision-
making, and there is a need for large-scale and fine-grained
investigations to extend current knowledge.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 9
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The datasets for this article are not publicly available because of
restrictions specified in the study consent-form, and conditions
for approval from the local ethics committee, concerning
patient confidentiality and participant privacy. Requests to
access the datasets should be directed to Jan Stubberud,
ETHICS STATEMENT
The studies involving human participants were reviewed
and approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and
Health Research Ethics, South-Eastern Norway (2017/666). The
patients/participants provided their written informed consent to
participate in this study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
BH completed the data collection and data curation,
analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript draft. The
study was part of a doctoral thesis by BH. JS provided
supervision, conceptualized the original trial, acted as principal
investigator, and contributed with revisions of the manuscript
draft. BL and NL contributed to the conceptualization of
the original trial and revision of the manuscript draft.
EK contributed with revisions of the manuscript draft.
All authors contributed to the article and approved the
submitted version.
FUNDING
This study was funded by the South-Eastern Norway Regional
Health Authority (Grant No. 2019120) and the research fund at
the Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital. The funding sources were not
otherwise involved in the research.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank all participants for their contributions.
Also, we thank Milada Cvancarova Småstuen (Lovisenberg
Diaconal Hospital, Oslo Metropolitan University) for
contributions to the statistical analysis and the results section
of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
Ahern, E., and Semkovska, M. (2017). Cognitive functioning in the
first-episode of major depressive disorder: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 31, 52–72. doi: 10.1037/neu00
00319
Barkham, M., Margison, F., Leach, C., Lucock, M., Mellor-Clark, J., Evans,
C., et al. (2001). Service profiling and outcomes benchmarking using
the CORE-OM: toward practice-based evidence in the psychological
therapies. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 69, 184–196. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.69.
2.184
Baune, B. T., Miller, R., McAfoose, J., Johnson, M., Quirk, F., and Mitchell,
D. (2010). The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in
major depression. Psychiatry Res. 176, 183–189. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.
12.001
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the
Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin. Psychol. Rev.
8, 77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An
Inventory for Measuring Depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi:
10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004
Buonocore, M., Bosia, M., Bechi, M., Spangaro, M., Cavedoni, S., Cocchi, F.,
et al. (2017). Is longer treatment better? A comparison study of 3 versus 6
months cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 31, 467–473.
doi: 10.1037/neu0000347
Chelune, G. J., Naugle, R. I., Lüders, H., Sedlak, J., and Awad, I. A. (1993).
Individual change after epilepsy surgery: practice effects and base-rate
information. Neuropsychology 7, 41–52. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.7.1.41
Conners, C. K. (2015). Conners Continuous Performance Test 3rd
editionTM(Conners’ CPT 3TM), Conners’ Continuous Auditory Test of Attention
(Conners’ CATATM) Manual. Canada: Multi-Health Systems Incorporated.
Davis, R. N., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility
among ruminators and nonruminators. Cogn. Ther. Res. 24,
699–711.
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function
System (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. A. (2000). CVLT-
II: California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version. San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation.
Duff, K. (2012). Evidence-based indicators of neuropsychological
change in the individual patient: relevant concepts and methods.
Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 27, 248–261. doi: 10.1093/arclin/
acr120
Elgamal, S., Denburg, S., Marriott, M., and MacQueen, G. (2010).
Clinical factors that predict cognitive function in patients with major
depression. Canad. J. Psychiatry 55, 653–661. doi: 10.1177/0706743710055
01004
Groves, S. J., Douglas, K. M., and Porter, R. J. (2018). A systematic
review of cognitive predictors of treatment outcome in major
depression. Front. in Psychiatry 9:382. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.
00382
Hagen, B. I., Lau, B., Joormann, J., Småstuen, M. C., Landrø, N. I., and Stubberud,
J. (2020). Goal Management Training as a cognitive remediation intervention
in depression: a randomized controlled trial. J. Affect. Disord. 275, 267–277.
doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.015
Hamilton, K. E., and Dobson, K. S. (2002). Cognitive therapy of depression:
pretreatment patient predictors of outcome. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 22, 875–893.
doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00106-X
Hasselbalch, B. J., Knorr, U., and Kessing, L. V. (2011). Cognitive
impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: a
systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 134, 20–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.
11.011
Huddy, V., Reeder, C., Kontis, D., Wykes, T., and Stahl, D. (2012). The effect
of working alliance on adherence and outcome in cognitive remediation
therapy. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 200, 614–619. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0b013e31
825bfc31
Jacobson, N. S., and Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: a statistical approach to
defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
59, 12–19. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.59.1.12
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: The Program of the Stress Reduction
Clinic at the University of Massachusetts Medical Center. New York, NY:
Delacorte Press.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 10
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
Kaser, M., Zaman, R., and Sahakian, B. J. (2017). Cognition as a treatment
target in depression. Psychol. Med. 47, 987–989. doi: 10.1017/s0033291716
003123
Koster, E. H. W., Hoorelbeke, K., Onraedt, T., Owens, M., and Derakshan, N.
(2017). Cognitive control interventions for depression: a systematic review of
findings from training studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 53, 79–92. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.
2017.02.002
Kurtz, M. M., Seltzer, J. C., Fujimoto, M., Shagan, D. S., and Wexler,
B. E. (2009). Predictors of change in life skills in schizophrenia after
cognitive remediation. Schizophr. Res. 107, 267–274. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.
10.014
Levine, B., Schweizer, T. A., O’Connor, C., Turner, G., Gillingham, S., Stuss, D. T.,
et al. (2011). Rehabilitation of executive functioning in patients with frontal
lobe brain damage with goal management training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:9.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00009
Lewandowski, K. E., Sperry, S. H., Cohen, B. M., Norris, L. A., Fitzmaurice,
G. M., Ongur, D., et al. (2017). Treatment to enhance cognition in
bipolar disorder (TREC-BD): efficacy of a randomized controlled trial
of cognitive remediation versus active control. J. Clin. Psychiatry 78,
e1242–e1249.
Lindenmayer, J. P., Fregenti, S., Kang, G., Ozog, V., Ljuri, I., Khan, A.,
et al. (2017). The relationship of cognitive improvement after cognitive
remediation with social functioning in patients with schizophrenia and severe
cognitive deficits. Schizophr. Res. 185, 154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.
01.007
Listunova, L., Bartolovic, M., Kienzle, J., Jaehn, A., Grützner, T. M., Wolf, R. C.,
et al. (2020). Predictors of cognitive remediation therapy improvement in
(partially) remitted unipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 264, 40–49. doi:
10.1016/j.jad.2019.12.006
MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., and Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the
practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol. Methods 7, 19–40.
doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.19
Marazziti, D., Consoli, G., Picchetti, M., Carlini, M., and Faravelli, L. (2010).
Cognitive impairment in major depression. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 626, 83–86.
Medalia, A., and Richardson, R. (2005). What predicts a good response to cognitive
remediation interventions? Schizophr. Bull. 31, 942–953. doi: 10.1093/schbul/
sbi045
Morimoto, S. S., Wexler, B. E., Liu, J., Hu, W., Seirup, J., and Alexopoulos,
G. S. (2014). Neuroplasticity-based computerized cognitive remediation for
treatment-resistant geriatric depression. Nat. Commun. 5, 1–7. doi: 10.1038/
ncomms5579
Motter, J. N., Pimontel, M. A., Rindskopf, D., Devanand, D. P.,
Doraiswamy, P. M., and Sneed, J. R. (2016). Computerized cognitive
training and functional recovery in major depressive disorder: a
meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 189, 184–191. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.
09.022
Petersen, J. Z., Porter, R. J., and Miskowiak, K. W. (2019). Clinical characteristics
associated with the discrepancy between subjective and objective cognitive
impairment in depression. J. Affect. Disord. 246, 763–774. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.
2018.12.105
Philippot, P., and Brutoux, F. (2008). Induced rumination dampens executive
processes in dysphoric young adults. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 39, 219–227.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.07.001
Porter, R. J., Bourke, C., Carter, J. D., Douglas, K. M., McIntosh, V. V. W.,
Jordan, J., et al. (2016). No change in neuropsychological dysfunction or
emotional processing during treatment of major depression with cognitive–
behaviour therapy or schema therapy. Psychol. Med. 46, 393–404. doi: 10.1017/
s0033291715001907
Pu, S., Noda, T., Setoyama, S., and Nakagome, K. (2018). Empirical evidence
for discrete neurocognitive subgroups in patients with non-psychotic major
depressive disorder: clinical implications. Psychol. Med. 48, 2717–2729. doi:
10.1017/s003329171800034x
Quinn, M. E., Keil, D. C., Utke, S., and Joormann, J. (2014). Trait rumination
moderates the effect of executive control training. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 5,
289–301. doi: 10.5127/jep.038713
Reser, M. P., Slikboer, R., and Rossell, S. L. (2019). A systematic review of factors
that influence the efficacy of cognitive remediation therapy in schizophrenia.
Austral. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 53, 624–641. doi: 10.1177/00048674198
53348
Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., and Blackwell, A. D. (2014). Cognitive
impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med.
44, 2029–2040. doi: 10.1017/s0033291713002535
Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. K., and Gioia, G. (2005). Behavioral Rating Inventory
of Executive Function–Adult Version. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Schulz, K. F., Altman, D. G., and Moher, D. (2010). CONSORT 2010 statement:
updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMC Med.
8:18. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-8-18
Seccomandi, B., Tsapekos, D., Newbery, K., Wykes, T., and Cella, M. (2020).
A systematic review of moderators of cognitive remediation response for
people with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 19:100160. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2019.
100160
Shilyansky, C., Williams, L. M., Gyurak, A., Harris, A., Usherwood, T., and Etkin, A.
(2016). Effect of antidepressant treatment on cognitive impairments associated
with depression: a randomised longitudinal study. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 425–435.
doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(16)00012-2
Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., and Thase, M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral therapies in
the 21st century: summary of an emerging field and an extended example of
cognitive control training for depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 31, 235–262. doi:
10.1007/s10608-006-9118-6
Siegle, G. J., Price, R. B., Jones, N. P., Ghinassi, F., Painter, T., and Thase,
M. E. (2014). You gotta work at it: pupillary indices of task focus are
prognostic for response to a neurocognitive intervention for rumination
in depression. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 455–471. doi: 10.1177/2167702614
536160
Snyder, H. R. (2012). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad
impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a
meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 81–132. doi: 10.1037/a00
28727
Stubberud, J., Langenbahn, D., Levine, B., Stanghelle, J., and Schanke, A.-K. (2013).
Goal management training of executive functions in patients with spina bifida:
a randomized controlled trial. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 19, 672–685. doi:
10.1017/s1355617713000209
Tornås, S., Løvstad, M., Solbakk, A.-K., Evans, J., Endestad, T., Hol,
P. K., et al. (2016). Rehabilitation of executive functions in patients
with chronic acquired brain injury with goal management training,
external cuing, and emotional regulation: a randomized controlled
trial. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 22, 436–452. doi: 10.1017/s1355617715
001344
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination
reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cogn. Ther. Res. 27, 247–259.
Twamley, E. W., Burton, C. Z., and Vella, L. (2011). Compensatory cognitive
training for psychosis: who benefits? Who stays in treatment? Schizophr. Bull.
37(Suppl._2), 55–62. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr059
Velligan, D. I., Kern, R. S., and Gold, J. M. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation for
schizophrenia and the putative role of motivation and expectancies. Schizophr.
Bull. 32, 474–485. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj071
Venezia, R. G., Gorlyn, M., Burke, A. K., Oquendo, M. A., Mann, J. J., and Keilp,
J. G. (2018). The impact of cognitive reserve on neurocognitive performance
in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 270, 211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.
psychres.2018.09.031
Vicent-Gil, M., Keymer-Gausset, A., Serra-Blasco, M., Carceller-Sindreu,
M., de Diego-Adeliño, J., Trujols, J., et al. (2018). Cognitive predictors
of illness course at 12 months after first-episode of depression. Eur.
Neuropsychopharmacol. 28, 529–537. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.
02.001
Victor, M., Lau, B., and Ruud, T. (2016). Patient characteristics in a return to work
programme for common mental disorders: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public
Health 16:745. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3431-0
Vita, A., Deste, G., De Peri, L., Barlati, S., Poli, R., Cesana, B. M.,
et al. (2013). Predictors of cognitive and functional improvement
and normalization after cognitive remediation in patients with
schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 150, 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.
08.011
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232
fpsyg-11-02232 September 9, 2020 Time: 22:5 # 11
Hagen et al. Predictors of Cognitive Remediation Outcome
Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the Wechsler abbreviated intelligence scale (WASI).
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2014). Wechsler adult intelligence scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV).
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
World Health Organization (2004). ICD-10: International Statistical Classification
of Diseases and Related Health Problems: Tenth Revision, 2nd Edn. Geneva:
World Health Organization.
Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., and Czobor, P. (2011). A meta-
analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect
sizes. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 472–485. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855
Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a
potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2020 Hagen, Landrø, Lau, Koster and Stubberud. This is an open-access
article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
(CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided
the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No
use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 11 September 2020 | Volume 11 | Article 2232